Advertisement

Hand-Held Object Force Direction Identification Thresholds at Rest and during Movement

  • Gabriel Baud-Bovy
  • Elia Gatti
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 6192)

Abstract

This study measured the minimum amount of force necessary to identify its direction. The force was produced by a robot and transmitted to a small spherical handle held between the thumb and index finger. We also examined whether this threshold changed during movement. We found that the force threshold was lower when it was possible to move the arm (5 g) than when it was immobile (10 g).

Keywords

force perception kinesthesia touch grasping 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Weber, E.H.: The sense of touch. Academic Press, London (1834/1978)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Baud-Bovy, G., Schochia, L.: Is mass invariant? Effects of movement amplitude and duration. In: Proc. 25th Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics, Galway, Ireland, pp. 369–374 (2009)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Jones, L.A.: Perception of Force and Weight: Theory and Research. Psychological Bulletin 100(1), 29–42 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Voerman, V.F., van Egmond, J., Crul, B.J.: Normal values for sensory thresholds in the cervical dermatomes: a critical note on the use of Semmes–Weinstein monofilaments. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 78, 24–29 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Vitello, M.P., Ernst, M.O., Fritschi, M.: An instance of tactile suppression: Active exploration impairs tactile sensitivity for the direction of lateral movement. In: Proc. EuroHaptics 2006, vol. 351, p. 355 (2006)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Brodie, E.E., Ross, H.E.: Jiggling a lifted weight does aid discrimination. The American Journal of Psychology 98, 469–471 (1985)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Zadeh, M.H., Wang, D., Kubica, E.: Perception-based lossy haptic compression considerations for velocity-based interactions. Multimedia Systems 13, 275–282 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Bocca, F., Baud-Bovy, G.: A model of perception of the central point of elastic force fields. In: 3rd Joint EuroHaptics Conf. and Symp. on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, Salt-Lake City, USA, pp. 576–581 (2009)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., Kakuda, N.: Tactile directional sensibility: peripheral neural mechanisms in man. Brain Research 866, 178–187 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Birznieks, I., Jenmalm, P., Goodwin, A.W., Johansson, R.S.: Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents. Journal of Neuroscience 21(20), 8222–8237 (2001)Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Pare, M., Carnahan, H., Smith, A.M.: Magnitude estimation of tangential force applied to the fingerpad. Experimental Brain Research 142, 342–348 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Jami, L.: Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. Psychological Review 72(3), 623–666 (1992)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Matthews, P.B.C.: Where does Sherrington’s “Muscular Sense” originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges? Annual Review of Neuroscience 5, 189–218 (1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Proske, U., Gandevia, S.C.: The kinaesthetic senses. J. Physiol. 587(17), 4139–4146 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    McCloskey, D.I.: Corollary discharges and motor commands. In: Handbook of Physiology The Nervous System III, Motor Control, Bethesda, pp. 1415–1448 (1981)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gabriel Baud-Bovy
    • 1
    • 2
  • Elia Gatti
    • 1
  1. 1.Vita-Salute San Raffaele UniversityMilanItaly
  2. 2.IIT Network Research, Unit of Molecular Neuroscience, San Raffaele FoundationMilanItaly

Personalised recommendations