Skip to main content

Handlungsfehler und Handlungsüberwachung

  • Chapter
Handlungspsychologie

Part of the book series: Springer-Lehrbuch ((SLB))

  • 5857 Accesses

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 29.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 34.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Weiterführende Literatur

  • Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8,539–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D. (1993). Die Logik des Misslingens – strategisches Denken in komplexen Situationen. Reinbek: Rowohlt. Science, 8,539–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. (1994). Menschliches Versagen: Psychologische Risikofaktoren und moderne Technologien. Heidelberg: Spektrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M, Doya, K. & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 593–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M. & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Literatur

  • Blakemore, S.-J., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(5), 551–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M. & Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1,635–640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108,624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K. & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doya, K. (2000). Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 732–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T. & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms re- solve conflict through cortical amplification of task rele- vant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoorman, J. & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components: II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78, 447– 455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row & Peterson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1904). Zur Psychopathologie des Alltagslebens. Berlin: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E. & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H. & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 4,480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawato, M. (1990) Feedback-error-learning neural network for supervised learning. In R. Eckmiller (ed.), Advanced neural computing(pp. 365–372). Amsterdam: North- Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald III, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A. et al. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303,1023–1026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A. & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior.New York: Holt, Reinhart & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. H. & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38,752–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. J. Davidson, G., G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and selfregulation: Advances in research and theory, vol. 4(pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. A. (2002). Consciousness is slower than you think. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1081– 1092.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. A., Cumming, G. & Vyas, S. M. (1978). Some errors of perceptual analysis in visual search can be detected and corrected. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 319–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Rodgers, B. (1977). What does a man do after he makes an error? An analysis of response programming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 727–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Vyas, S. M. (1981). Processing a display even after you make a response to it. How perceptual errors can be corrected. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? In K. D. Duncan, M. M. Gruenberg & D. Wallis (eds.), Changes in working life(pp. 97–113). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. (1979). Actions not as planned:The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (eds.), Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1. Psychological issue(pp. 51–74). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, M. A. & Wurtz, R. H. (2006). Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature, 444, 374–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K. & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 94, 14792–14797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Holst, E. (1954). Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. The British Journal of Animal Behavior, 2,89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hommel, B., Nattkemper, D. (2011). Handlungsfehler und Handlungsüberwachung. In: Handlungspsychologie. Springer-Lehrbuch. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-12858-5_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics