Advertisement

Attention Mechanisms in Behavioral Inhibition: Exploring and Exploiting the Environment

Chapter

Abstract

Attention mechanisms may play a pivotal role in the emergence of socioemotional profiles. In the case of behavioral inhibition (BI), sensitivity to novelty, coupled with an attention bias to threat, may increase the risk for socially reticent behavior and anxiety. Early work suggests that behaviorally inhibited children may be more vigilant to novelty and threat in the environment, which then leads to behavioral avoidance. In addition, attention bias to threat, in particular, may moderate the relation between early behavioral inhibition and the later emergence of anxiety. However, we are only now examining how these early attentional processes act to shape observed outcomes. The current chapter speculates that attention mechanisms may lead behaviorally inhibited children to resort to habitual and inflexible repertoires for dealing with unfamiliar and unexpected environments. Thus, rather than explore new contexts, which may lead to new knowledge and reward, behaviorally inhibited children may instead exploit prior knowledge and behaviors, limiting their exposure to new experiences. The lack of new experiences, in turn, decreases the probability that behaviorally inhibited children will recognize that their initial fears and concerns are often unwarranted.

Keywords

Behavioral inhibition Attention Anxiety Exploration and exploitation 

References

  1. Aktar, E., Majdandžić, M., De Vente, W., & Bögels, S. M. (2017). Parental expressions of anxiety and child temperament in toddlerhood jointly predict preschoolers’ avoidance of novelty. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 29, 1–14.Google Scholar
  2. Aktar, E., Mandell, D. J., de Vente, W., Majdandžić, M., Raijmakers, M. E., & Bögels, S. M. (2016). Infants’ temperament and mothers’, and fathers’ depression predict infants’ attention to objects paired with emotional faces. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 975–990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Almas, A. N., Phillips, D. A., Henderson, H. A., Hane, A. A., Degnan, K. A., Moas, O. L., & Fox, N. A. (2011). The relations between infant negative reactivity, non-maternal childcare, and children’s interactions with familiar and unfamiliar peers. Social Development, 20, 718–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00605.x CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Amso, D., & Scerif, G. (2015). The attentive brain: Insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 606–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Anaya, B., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (in press). Personality development in the context of temperament and parenting dynamics. New Ideas in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.03.002
  6. Auday, E., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2018). Neural correlates of attention bias to masked facial threat cues: Examining children at-risk for social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 19, 202–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48, 662–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Friedman, D., Robin, J. A., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2001). Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in high-risk children of parents with panic disorder and major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.49 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brooker, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Leve, L. D., Shaw, D. S., & Reiss, D. (2014). Birth and adoptive parent anxiety symptoms moderate the link between infant attention control and internalizing problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 347–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brooker, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Kiel, E. J., Leve, L. D., Shaw, D. S., & Reiss, D. (2011). The association between infants’ attention control and social inhibition is moderated by genetic and environmental risk for anxiety. Infancy, 16, 490–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buss, K. A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should we worry about? Context, fear regulation, and anxiety risk. Developmental Psychology, 47, 804–819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buss, K. A., Davis, E. L., Kiel, E. J., Brooker, R. J., Beekman, C., & Early, M. C. (2013). Dysregulated fear predicts social wariness and social anxiety symptoms during kindergarten. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42(5), 603–616. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.769170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cameron, J. L., Eagleson, K. L., Fox, N. A., Hensch, T. K., & Levitt, P. (2017). Social origins of developmental risk for mental and physical illness. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 10783–10791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Capitanio, J. P. (2017). Naturally occurring nonhuman primate models of psychosocial processes. ILAR Journal, 58, 226–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cavigelli, S. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2003). Fear of novelty in infant rats predicts adult corticosterone dynamics and an early death. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 16131–16136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cavigelli, S. A., Stine, M. M., Kovacsics, C., Jefferson, A., Diep, M. N., & Barrett, C. E. (2007). Behavioral inhibition and glucocorticoid dynamics in a rodent model. Physiology & Behavior, 92, 897–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (2013). Goodness of fit: Clinical applications, from infancy through adult life. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  18. Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The development of anxiety: The role of control in the early environment. Psychological Bulletin, 124(1), 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., … Fox, N. A. (2009). Stable behavioral inhibition during infancy and early childhood predicts the development of anxiety disorders in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 928–935. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.ob013e3181ae09df CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Clauss, J. A., Avery, S. N., VanDerKlok, R. M., Rogers, B. P., Cowan, R. L., Benningfield, M. M., & Blackford, J. U. (2014). Neurocircuitry underlying risk and resilience to social anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 31, 822–833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Clauss, J. A., & Blackford, J. U. (2012). Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 1066–1075. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.002 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Cohen, M. M., Jing, D., Yang, R. R., Tottenham, N., Lee, F. S., & Casey, B. J. (2013). Early-life stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 18274–18278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Colombo, J., & Salley, B. (2015). Biopsychosocial perspectives on the development of attention in infancy. In S. B. Calkins (Ed.), Handbook of infant biopsychosocial development (pp. 71–96). New York, NY: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
  25. Coplan, R. J., Rubin, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Stewart, S. L. (1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: Distinguishing among reticence and passive and active solitude in young children. Child Development, 65, 129–137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00739.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Cremone, A., Kurdziel, L. B., Fraticelli-Torres, A., McDermott, J. M., & Spencer, R. (2017). Napping reduces emotional attention bias during early childhood. Developmental Science, 20(4). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12411 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Degnan, K. A., & Fox, N. A. (2007). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: Multiple levels of a resilience process. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 729–746. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000363 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Walker, O. L., Ghera, M. M., & Fox, N. A. (2015). Emergent patterns of risk for psychopathology: The influence of infant avoidance and maternal caregiving on trajectories of social reticence. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 1163–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Diorio, J., & Meaney, M. J. (2007). Maternal programming of defensive responses through sustained effects on gene expression. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 32, 275–284.Google Scholar
  30. Dodd, H. F., Vogt, J., Turkileri, N., & Notebaert, L. (2017). Task relevance of emotional information affects anxiety-linked attention bias in visual search. Biological Psychology, 122, 13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Dudeney, J., Sharpe, L., & Hunt, C. (2015). Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 66–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ehlers, M. R., & Todd, R. M. (2017). Genesis and maintenance of attentional biases: The role of the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system. Neural Plasticity, 2017, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75, 334–339. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00674.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Individual differences in executive attention predict self-regulation and adolescent psychosocial behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 337–340. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1308.041 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Field, A. (2006). Watch out for the beast: Fear information and attentional biases in children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 35, 431–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Fox, N. A., Hane, A. A., & Pine, D. S. (2007). Plasticity for affective neurocircuitry: How the environment affects gene expression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 235–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R., Coplan, R. J., Porges, S. W., … Stewart, S. (1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Development, 66, 1771–1784. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00964.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 1155–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gao, W., Zhu, H., Giovanello, K. S., Smith, J. K., Shen, D., Gilmore, J. H., & Lin, W. (2009). Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 6790–6795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. García Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130152 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., … Tottenham, N. (2013). A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala–prefrontal circuitry. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4584–4593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Grupe, D. W., & Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: An integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 488–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychological Science, 17, 550–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2016). Early caregiving and human biobehavioral development: A comparative physiology approach. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 7, 82–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2016). An integrative network approach to social anxiety disorder: The complex dynamic interplay among attentional bias for threat, attentional control, and symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 42, 95–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Henderson, H. A. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control and the regulation of shyness in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 177–193. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903526538 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Henderson, H. A., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2015). Behavioral inhibition and developmental risk: A dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 207–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Henderson, H. A., & Wilson, M. J. (2017). Attention processes underlying risk and resilience in behaviorally inhibited children. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 4, 99–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2010). The development of emotional face and eye gaze processing. Developmental Science, 13(6), 813–825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hummel, A. C., Premo, J. E., & Kiel, E. J. (2017). Attention to threat as a predictor of shyness in the context of internalizing and externalizing behavior. Infancy, 22, 240–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Telzer, E. H., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Goff, B., Flannery, J., & Tottenham, N. (2015). Exploration—Exploitation strategy is dependent on early experience. Developmental Psychobiology, 57, 313–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child Development, 68, 139–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Kagan, J. (2012). The biography of behavioral inhibition. In M. Zentner & R. L. Shiner (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 69–82). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  56. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130685 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Kagan, J., Snidman, N., McManis, M., & Woodward, S. (2001). Temperamental contributions to the affect family of anxiety. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24(4), 677–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70257-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Kayser, A. S., Mitchell, J. M., Weinstein, D., & Frank, M. J. (2015). Dopamine, locus of control, and the exploration-exploitation tradeoff. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 454–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Kiel, E. J., & Buss, K. A. (2010). Maternal expectations for toddlers' reactions to novelty: Relations of maternal internalizing symptoms and parenting dimensions to expectations and accuracy of expectations. Parenting: Science and Practice, 10, 202–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Kiel, E. J., & Buss, K. A. (2011). Toddlers’ duration of attention toward putative threat. Infancy, 16, 198–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Kiel, E. J., & Buss, K. A. (2012). Associations among context-specific maternal protective behavior, toddlers’ fearful temperament, and maternal accuracy and goals. Social Development, 21, 742–760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kiel, E. J., & Buss, K. A. (2014). Dysregulated fear in toddlerhood predicts kindergarten social withdrawal through protective parenting. Infant and Child Development, 23, 304–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Kiel, E. J., & Hummel, A. C. (2017). Contextual influences on concordance between maternal report and laboratory observation of toddler fear. Emotion, 17, 240–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Kiel, E. J., Premo, J. E., & Buss, K. A. (2016). Maternal encouragement to approach novelty: A curvilinear relation to change in anxiety for inhibited toddlers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 433–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Lau, J. Y., & Waters, A. M. (2017). Annual Research Review: An expanded account of information-processing mechanisms in risk for child and adolescent anxiety and depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Laureiro-Martínez, D., Brusoni, S., & Zollo, M. (2010). The neuroscientific foundations of the exploration−exploitation dilemma. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 3, 95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Leppänen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Lima, S. L., & Dill, L. M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68, 619–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Liu, P., Taber-Thomas, B. C., Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2018). Biobehavioral markers of attention bias modification in temperamental risk for anxiety: A randomized control trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 57, 103–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. LoBue, V., Rakison, D., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Threat perception across the lifespan: Evidence for multiple converging pathways. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 375–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. McEwen, B. S., & Morrison, J. H. (2013). The brain on stress: Vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron, 79, 16–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Meyer, A. (2017). A biomarker of anxiety in children and adolescents: A review focusing on the error-related negativity (ERN) and anxiety across development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 58–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Meyer, A., Weinberg, A., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The development of the error-related negativity (ERN) and its relationship with anxiety: Evidence from 8 to 13 year-olds. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 152–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Morales, S., Beekman, C., Blandon, A. Y., Stifter, C. A., & Buss, K. A. (2015). Longitudinal associations between temperament and socioemotional outcomes in young children: The moderating role of RSA and gender. Developmental Psychobiology, 57, 105–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Morales, S., Brown, K. M., Taber-Thomas, B. C., LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2017). Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion, 17, 874–883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Morales, S., Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect biased attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 26–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Morales, S., Pérez-Edgar, K., & Buss, K. A. (2015). Attention biases towards and away from threat mark the relation between early dysregulated fear and the later emergence of social withdrawal. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1067–1078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Morales, S., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2017). Patterns of attention to threat across tasks in behaviorally inhibited children at risk for anxiety. Developmental Science, 20, e12391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 71–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Nozadi, S. S., Troller-Renfree, S., White, L. K., Frenkel, T., Degnan, K. A., Bar-Haim, Y., … Fox, N. A. (2016). The moderating role of attention biases in understanding the link between behavioral inhibition and anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 7, 451–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Peltola, M. J., Hietanen, J. K., Forssman, L., & Leppänen, J. M. (2013). The emergence and stability of the attentional bias to fearful faces in infancy. Infancy, 18(6), 905–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Peltola, M. J., Leppanen, J. M., Maki, S., & Hietanen, J. K. (2009). Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 134–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Peltola, M. J., Leppanen, J. M., Palokangas, T., & Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Fearful faces modulate looking duration and attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 11, 60–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., & Leppänen, J. M. (2018). Infants’ attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Developmental Science, e12687. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12687
  86. Pérez-Edgar, K. (2015). Effortful control in adolescence: Individual differences within a unique developmental window. In G. Oettingen & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Self-regulation in adolescence (pp. 78–102). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Pérez-Edgar, K., Bar-Haim, Y., McDermott, J. M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal. Emotion, 10, 349–357. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018486 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  88. Pérez-Edgar, K., & Fox, N. A. (2005). Temperament and anxiety disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 681–706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2005.05.008 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Pérez-Edgar, K., & Guyer, A. E. (2014). Behavioral inhibition: Temperament or prodrome? Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 1(3), 182–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Pérez-Edgar, K., McDermott, J. M., Korelitz, K., Degnan, K. A., Curby, T. W., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Patterns of sustained attention in infancy shape the developmental trajectory of social behavior from toddlerhood through adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1723–1730. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021064 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  91. Pérez-Edgar, K., Morales, S., LoBue, V., Taber-Thomas, B. C., Allen, E. K., Brown, K. M., & Buss, K. A. (2017). The impact of negative affect on attention patterns to threat across the first two years of life. Developmental Psychology, 53, 2219–2232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Pérez-Edgar, K., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., McDermott, J. M., White, L. K., Henderson, H. A., Degnan, K. A., … Fox, N. A. (2011). Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 885–895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9495-5 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  93. Pérez-Edgar, K., Taber-Thomas, B., Auday, E., & Morales, S. (2014). Temperament and attention as core mechanisms in the early emergence of anxiety. In K. Lagattuta (Ed.), Children and emotion: New insights into developmental affective science (Vol. 26, pp. 42–56). Basel, Switzerland: Karger Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2012). Control networks and neuromodulators of early development. Developmental Psychology, 48, 827–835. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025530 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Reader, S. M. (2015). Causes of individual differences in animal exploration and search. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7, 451–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., Vanderwert, R. E., Degnan, K. A., Marshall, P. J., Pérez-Edgar, K., Chronis-Tuscano, A., … Fox, N. A. (2009). Attention to novelty in behaviorally inhibited adolescents moderates risk for anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1365–1372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Rossi, V., & Pourtois, G. (2017). Someone’s lurking in the dark: The role of state anxiety on attention deployment to threat-related stimuli. Biological Psychology, 122, 21–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1113–1143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3, 207–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910387943 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  100. Roy, A. K., Dennis, T. A., & Warner, C. M. (2015). A critical review of attentional threat bias and its role in the treatment of pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(3), 171–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Rubin, K., Chen, X., & Hymel, S. (1993). Socioemotional characteristics of withdrawn and aggressive children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 518–534.Google Scholar
  102. Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Sternberg, E. M., Gold, P. W., Smith, C. C., & Schulkin, J. (1997). Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 127–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). Behavioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation in temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 119–135. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199909)35:2<119::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-G CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Scholer, A. A., Zou, X., Fujita, K., Stroessner, S. J., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). When risk seeking becomes a motivational necessity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 215–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Shamir-Essakow, G., Ungerer, J. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Attachment, behavioral inhibition, and anxiety in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 131–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 2–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., Franke, D., Pope, D. L. W., Snyder, A. Z., McAvoy, M. P., & Corbetta, M. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 4392–4407. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5609-08.2009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Suor, J. H., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cicchetti, D. (2017). A life history approach to delineating how harsh environments and hawk temperament traits differentially shape children’s problem-solving skills. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 902–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Susa, G., Pitică, I., Benga, O., & Miclea, M. (2012). The self regulatory effect of attentional control in modulating the relationship between attentional biases toward threat and anxiety symptoms in children. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 1069–1083. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.638910 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Sylvester, C. M., Hudziak, J. J., Gaffrey, M. S., Barch, D. M., & Luby, J. L. (2016). Stimulus-driven attention, threat bias, and sad bias in youth with a history of an anxiety disorder or depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 219–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Taber-Thomas, B., Morales, S., Hillary, F., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2016). Altered topography of intrinsic functional connectivity in childhood risk for social anxiety. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 995–1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Todd, R. M., Cunningham, W. A., Anderson, A. K., & Thompson, E. (2012). Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 365–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Toelch, U., Bruce, M. J., Meeus, M. T., & Reader, S. M. (2011). Social performance cues induce behavioral flexibility in humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Tottenham, N. (2013). The importance of early experiences for neuro-affective development. In The neurobiology of childhood (pp. 109–129). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Verbeek, M. E., Drent, P. J., & Wiepkema, P. R. (1994). Consistent individual differences in early exploratory behaviour of male great tits. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1113–1121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2013). Competing for attentional priority: Temporary goals versus threats. Emotion, 13(3), 587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Wass, S. V., Scerif, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2012). Training attentional control and working memory—Is younger, better? Developmental Review, 32, 360–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1996). Modeling cognition in emotional disorder: The S-REF model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 881–888. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00050-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. White, L. K., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., Pérez-Edgar, K., Walker, O. L., Schechner, T., … Fox, N. A. (2017). Developmental relations between behavioral inhibition, anxiety, and attention biases to threat and positive information. Child Development, 88, 141–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Williams, L. R., Degnan, K. A., Pérez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Pine, D. S., … Fox, N. A. (2009). Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 1063–1075. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9331-3 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  121. Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2014). Brain electrical activity of shy and non-shy preschool-aged children during executive function tasks. Infant and Child Development, 23, 259–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe Pennsylvania State UniversityUniversity ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations