Skip to main content

Progress and Prospects in Habit Research

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychology of Habit

Abstract

Considerable progress has been made in clarifying theoretical understanding of habit. Research across diverse sub-disciplines including neuroscience, learning paradigms and social psychology has contributed to an understanding that habits are behaviours elicited by cues, and which may occur independently of goals or current motivational state. Cue dependence arises from behavioural repetition in the past and is represented in memory as a cue–response association. We draw together the different strands of this research to consider the relationship of habit to motivational state during development, enactment and suppression of habit. We consider the nature of habit measurement and call for greater attention to fundamental features of habit, namely cue dependence, history of repetition and goal independence. Finally we comment on the relationship of habit to its great antonym, willpower, and highlight the possible role of habit as a functional self-regulatory tool.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aarts, H. (2007). Health and goal-directed behaviour: The nonconscious regulation and motivation of goals and their pursuit. Health Psychology Review, 1, 53–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforce devaluation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 34, 77–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adriaanse, M., Gollwitzer, P. M., De Ridder, T. D., de Wit, J. B. F., & Kroese, F. M. (2011). Breaking habits with implementation intentions: A test of underlying processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 502–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adriaanse, M., Kroese, F. M., Gillebaart, M., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2014). Effortless inhibition: Habit mediates the relation between self-control and unhealthy snack consumption. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adriaanse, M., Kroese, F., Weijers, J., Gollwitzer, P., & Oettingen, G. (2018). Explaining unexplainable food choices. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, O15–O24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., & Nunn, R. G. (1973). Habit-reversal: A method of eliminating nervous habits and tics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11, 619–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2014). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). Behavioral indecision: Effects of self-focus on automatic behavior. Social Cognition, 18, 55–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersch, K. D., Lim, T.-V., Ward, L. H. E., Robbins, T. W., & Stochl, J. (2017). Creature of habit: A self-report measure of habitual routines and automatic tendencies in everyday life. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 508–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, B., Abraham, C., Lally, P., & de Bruijn, G.-J. (2012). Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the self-report habit index. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, B. (2015). A review and analysis of the use of ‘habit’ in understanding, predicting and influencing health-related behavior. Health Psychology Review, 9, 277–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, C. M., Biswas, S., Jung, D., Perin, J., Parvin, T., et al. (2017). Psychosocial factors mediating the effect of the CHoBI7 intervention on handwashing with soap: A randomized controlled trial. Health Education and Behavior, 44, 613–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillan, C. M., Otto, A. R., Phelps, E. A., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 523–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel, A. M., & Smith, K. S. (2014). Good habits, bad habits. Scientific American, 310, 38–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Forster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1318–1335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., & Gerstenberg, F. (2014). Exploring the interplay of trait self-control and ego depletion: Empirical evidence for ironic effects. European Journal of Personality, 28, 413–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itzchakov, G., Uziel, L., & Wood, W. (2018). When attitudes and habits don’t correspond: Self-control depletion increases persuasion but not behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 75, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1887). The laws of habit. The Popular Science Monthly, 31, 433–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ji, M. F., & Wood, W. (2007). Purchase and consumption habits: Not necessarily what you intend. Journal of Consumer Psychology (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 17, 261–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehéricy, S., Benali, H., Van de Moortele, P. F., Pélégrini-Issac, M., Waechter, T., et al. (2005). Distinct basal ganglia territories are engaged in early and advanced motor sequence learning. PNAS, 102, 12566–12571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, P.-Y., Wood, W., & Monterosso, J. (2016). Healthy eating habits protect against temptations. Appetite, 103, 432–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau, T. M., Hollands, G. J., & Kelly, M. P. (2015). Changing population behaviour and reducing health disparities: Exploring the potential of ‘Choice Architecture’ Interventions. In R. M. Kaplan, M. L. Spittel, & D. H. David (Eds.), Population health: Behavioral and social science insights. AHRQ Publication No.15-002 (pp. 105–126). Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research Quality and Office of Behavioral and Social Sciences Research, National Institutes of Health.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, E., Sheeran, P., Baird, H., Macdonald, I., Webb, T. L., & Harris, P. R. (2016). Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1075–1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittal, B. (1988). Achieving higher seat belt usage: The role of habit in bridging the attitude-behavior gap. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 993–1016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neal, D. T., Wood, W., & Drolet, A. (2013). How do people adhere to goals when willpower is low? The profits (and pitfalls) of strong habits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 959–975.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2012). How do habits guide behaviour? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 492–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Wu, M., & Kurlander, D. (2011). The pull of the past: When do habits persist despite conflict with motives? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(11), 1428–1437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orbell, S., & Phillips, S. A. (in press). Automatic processes in illness self-regulation. Health Psychology Review. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17437199.2018.1503559

  • Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (1998). ‘Inclined abstainers’: A problem for predicting health behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 151–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2010). The automatic component of habit in health behavior: Habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychology, 29, 374–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2015). The strength of habit. Health Psychology Review, 9, 311–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. A., & Gardner, B. (2016). Habitual exercise instigation (vs. execution) predicts healthy adults’ exercise frequency. Health Psychology, 35, 69–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, M. A., & Yin, H. H. (2012). Methods for studying habitual behavior in mice. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, 60, 8.29.1–8.29.9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., Godin, G., Conner, M., & Germain, M. (2017). Paradoxical effects of experience: Past behavior both strengthens and weakens the intention-behavior relationship. Journal of the Association of Consumer Research. Published online 17th April 2017. https://doi.org/10.1086/691216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tricomi, E., Balleine, B. W., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2009). A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 2225–2232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., & van Knippenberg, C. (1994). Attitude versus general habit: Antecedents of travel mode choice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Faes, S. (1999). Good intentions, bad habits, and effects of forming implementation intentions on healthy eating. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 591–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Friborg, O., Wang, C. E., Trafimow, D., & Woolf, K. (2007). Mental habits: Metacognitive reflection on negative self-thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), 526–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1313–1330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Roy, D. (2016). Empowering interventions to promote sustainable lifestyles: Testing the habit discontinuity hypothesis in a field experiment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 127–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Walker, I., Davis, A., & Jurasek, M. (2008). Context change and travel mode choice: Combining the habit discontinuity and self-activation hypotheses. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28, 121–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Wood, W. (2006). Interventions to break and create consumer habits. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 25, 90–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P., & de Wit, S. (2018). Current limits of experimental research into habits and future directions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20, 33–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Rünger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 11.1–11.26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., Tam, L., & Witt, M. G. (2005). Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 918–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 464–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Orbell, S., Verplanken, B. (2018). Progress and Prospects in Habit Research. In: Verplanken, B. (eds) The Psychology of Habit. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-97529-0_22

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics