Skip to main content

Ception and the Discrepancy Between Vision and Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Sensory Perceptions in Language, Embodiment and Epistemology

Part of the book series: Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics ((SAPERE,volume 42))

  • 330 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter deals with the sensory perception of vision and investigates the correlation between body, mind, and language in a corpus of English written descriptions of pictorial material. Expressions such as The track plunges down the mountain or The biceps muscle goes from the shoulder to the elbow represent a specific type of event verbalisation, which Talmy (1983) named ‘Fictive Motion’, whereby a degree of discrepancy exists between the visual experience of a stationary scene (track, muscle) and its linguistic description as a motion event (to plunge, to go). The production of such sentences requires the percipient/describer to mentally simulate motion along a path or linear configuration, although the subject noun phrase is a stationary entity. The frameworks of Cognitive Semantics (Talmy 2000) and Embodiment (Gallese and Lakoff 2005; Boulenger et al. 2008) along with the cognitively-oriented version of Construction Grammar (Goldberg 2006; Ruiz de Mendoza and Mairal Usón 2008) are the main theoretical approaches brought together (1) to address the category of General Fictivity and the Embodied Cognition Theory, (2) to analyse the syntactic patterns of Fictive Motion expressions, (3) to show the inconsistency of Matlock’s (2004) “binary typology”, and (4) to pin down the internal and external constraints that licence the wording of nonveridical motion events.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Examples such as Service stations are every now and then along the motorway show that fictive motion can be instantiated even in absence of a motion verb; it is the interpreter that builds a dynamic construal.

  2. 2.

    Talmy called implicit motion fictive motion (1983), Langacker (1986) abstract motion, and Matsumoto (1996) subjective motion.

  3. 3.

    The array of patterns show that the coding of motion is, as any other dimension of grammar, subject to competing motivations (i.e. semantico-pragmatic, discourse-functional). Examples (5) and (8) represent an iconic dimension (iconicity of distance) as well as information structure (Communicative Dynamism and the End Weight/End Focus principles) (see MacWhinney et al. 2014).

  4. 4.

    He calls factive the assessment of greater veridicality, while he calls fictive the assessment of greater nonveridicality. It is worth underlying that fictive is not to be equalled with fictitious, since it is not Talmy’s intention to suggest ‘objective unreality’ of the representation.

  5. 5.

    These are as follows: Clarity, strength, ostension, objectivity, localizability, identifiability, content/structure, type of geometry, accessibility to consciousness, certainty, actionability, and stimulus dependence.

  6. 6.

    Construal refers to the ability to perceive and describe the same situation in alternate ways by means of language choice: “Every lexical and grammatical element incorporates a particular way of construing conceptual content. It follows that a change in grammatical class involves a reconceptualization, and alternate construal resulting in a subtly different meaning in accordance with the abstract semantic values of the classes” (Langacker 2001: 8).

  7. 7.

    Evidentiality may occur, as in ‘The river appears to run through a tunnel in the mountain’; it expresses the describer’s degree of certainty about what s/he sees, perhaps because s/he is distant from what s/he sees (Chafe and Nichols 1986).

References

  • Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., et al. (2006). Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Current Biology, 16, 1818–1823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baicchi, A. (2011). Metaphoric motivation in grammatical structure. The case of the caused-motion construction. In K. U. Panther & G. Radden (Eds.), Motivation in grammar and the lexicon (pp. 149–170). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baicchi, A. (2015). Construction learning as a complex adaptive system. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., & Pulvermuller, F. (2008). Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1905–1914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, C. (2009). The lexical constructional model: Genesis, strengths and challenges. In C. Butler & J. Martín Arista (Eds.), Deconstructing constructions (pp. 117–152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, C. S., & Gonzálvez-García, F. (2014). Exploring functional-cognitive space. Amsterdam-Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W., & Nichols, J. (Eds.). (1986). Evidentiality: the linguistic coding of epistemology. Writing Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensori-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 455–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilchrist, A., & Rock I.(1981). Rational processes in perception. In Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive (pp. 50–56). Berkeley, CA: Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., et al. (2008). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 905–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez Martínez-Losa, N. (2009). Towards a typology of fictive motion events: Review of existing proposals and presentation of new perspectives. Interlingüística, 17, 562–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to the western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1986). An introduction to cognitive grammar. Cognitive Science, 10, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1999). Grammar and conceptualization. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (2001). Discourse in cognitive grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 143–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. (1996). Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, et al. (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 77–107). Cambridge MA.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., Malchukov, A., & Moravcsik, E. (Eds.). (2014). Competing motivations in grammar and usage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matlock, T. (2004). The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In G. Radden & R. Dirven (Eds.), Studies in linguistic motivation (pp. 221–248). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, Y. (1996). Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 138–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merlini, L. (2009). The speaker’s imprint in descriptive discourse. In S. Radighieri & P. Tucker (Eds.), Point of view: Description and evaluation across discourses (pp. 15–36). Roma: Officina edizioni.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1974). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pobric, G., & Hamilton, A. F. (2006). Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex. Current Biology, 16, 524–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radden, G., Köpcke, K., Berg, T., et al. (Eds.). (2007). Aspects of meaning construction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, D., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz de Mendoza, F., & Mairal Usón, R. (2008). Levels of description and constraining factors in meaning construction: An introduction to the lexical constructional model. Folia Linguistica, 42(2), 355–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J., Gumpers & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. (2003). Modes of discourse: The local structure of texts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In H. Pick & L. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial orientation. Theory, research, and application (pp. 225–282). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive motion in language and “ception”. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 273–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallentin, M., Lund, T. E., Ostergaard, S., et al. (2005). Motion verb sentences activate left posterior middle temporal cortex despite static context. NeuroReport, 16, 649–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welby, V. (1983 [1903]). What is meaning? Studies in the development of significance. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werlich, E. (1976). A text grammar of English. Heidelberg: Quelle und Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winawer, J., Huk, A. C., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). A motion aftereffect from still photographs depicting motion. Psychological Science, 19, 276–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annalisa Baicchi .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Baicchi, A. (2018). Ception and the Discrepancy Between Vision and Language. In: Baicchi, A., Digonnet, R., Sandford, J. (eds) Sensory Perceptions in Language, Embodiment and Epistemology. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 42. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91277-6_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics