Skip to main content

Perceived Informativity and Referential Effects of Contrast in Adjectivally Modified NPs

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Semantics of Gradability, Vagueness, and Scale Structure

Part of the book series: Language, Cognition, and Mind ((LCAM,volume 4))

  • 472 Accesses

Abstract

Referential Effects of Contrast (RECs) involving reference resolution of adjectivally modified NPs (e.g., the tall glass) have been attributed to pragmatic reasoning based on the informativity of modification (Sedivy et al. Cognition, 71(2):109–147, 1999; Sedivy, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1):3–23, 2003; Sedivy, Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 345–364, 2004, a.o.). Recently, it has been claimed that informativity alone cannot account for all the attested interactions between adjectival meaning and context and that factors related to efficiency in the search of a referent also play an important role (Rubio-Fernández, Frontiers in Psychology, 7(153), 2016). Building on Aparicio et al. (Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory, vol. 25, 2015), this paper demonstrates that perceived informativity plays an important role in RECs, but lexical semantic properties of different adjective classes are also relevant. We present results from a Visual World eye-tracking study which shows that adjective classes differ in whether they introduce RECs, and results from an offline judgment task which show that this difference correlates to some extent with the perceived informativity of members of these classes. Color adjectives, relative adjectives and maximum standard absolute adjectives were rated as overinformative when used as modifiers in the absence of contrast, and gave rise to RECs; minimum standard absolute adjectives were not rated as overinformative when used as modifiers in the absence of contrast, and did not give rise to RECs. Taken together, our results show that perceived informativity plays an important role in RECs. We also discuss additional differences between the adjective classes which suggest that differences in lexical semantics can further contribute to differences in RECs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Although color adjectives are both context dependent and vague, they are sensitive to different kinds of contextual parameters from RelAs and MaxAAs. See Rothschild and Segal (2009), Kennedy and McNally (2010), Clapp (2012) for discussion.

  2. 2.

    Several linguistic tests diagnose whether an absolute adjective makes use of a maximum versus minimum versus relative standard. For instance, Kennedy (2007) points out that these three classes give rise to different entailment patterns when used in comparatives. In comparatives of the form X is more A than Y, MinAAs entail that X is A (i); MaxAAs entail that B is not A (ii); and (unmarked) RelAs entail neither that X is (not) A nor that Y is (not) A (iii).

    figure a
    figure b
    figure c

    The distribution of modifiers like slightly and completely are also often described as tests for MinAA and MaxAA status, respectively, but strictly speaking, these modifiers test for minimum and maximum scalar endpoints, respectively, which are independent of—though generally correlated with—maximum and minimum standards.

  3. 3.

    See supplementary materials to this chapter for a full list of the experimental items used in Experiment 1.

  4. 4.

    See supplementary materials to this chapter for a full list of the experimental items used in Experiment 2.

References

  • Altmann, G., & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30(3), 191–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aparicio, H., Xiang, M., & Kennedy, C. (2015). Processing gradable adjectives in context: A visual world study. In S. D’Antonio, M. Moroney, & C. R. Little (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory. Publisher: Linguistic Society of America and Cornell Linguistics. CircleUrl: http://journals.linguisticsociety.org/proceedings/index.php/SALT/issue/view/132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arts, A., Maes, A., Noorman, K., & Jansen, C. (2011). Overspecification facilitates object identification. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(1), 361–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belke, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2002). Tracking the time course of multidimensional stimulus discrimination: Analyses of viewing patterns and processing times during “same”-“different” decisions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 237–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, H. (2014). A delineation solution to the puzzles of absolute adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 37(1), 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clapp, L. (2012). Indexical color predicates: Truth conditional semantics vs. truth conditional pragmatics. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 42(2), 71–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6(1), 84–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., & Steedman, M. (1985). On not being led up the garden path: the use of context by the psychological parser. In D. Dowty, L. Kartunnen, & A. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural Language Parsing. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard, K. M., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Sedivy, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1995). Eye movements as a window into real-time spoken language comprehension in natural contexts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(6), 409–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodner, D., & Sedivy, J. C. (2011). The effect of speaker-specific information on pragmatic inferences. In N. Pearlmutter & E. Gibson (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference (pp. 239–272). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hanna, J. E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Science, 28(1), 105–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanna, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. (2007). Vagueness and grammar: The semantics of relative and absolute gradable adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30(1), 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C., & McNally, L. (2005). Scale structure and the semantic typology of gradable predicates. Language, 81(2), 345–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C., & McNally, L. (2010). Color, context, and compositionality. Synthese, 174(1), 79–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koolen, R., Gatt, A., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2011). Factors causing overspecification in definite descriptions. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(13), 3231–3250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koolen, R., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2013). The effect of scene variatio on the redundant use of color in definite reference. Cognitive Science, 37(2), 395–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lassiter, D., & Goodman, N. D. (2013). Context, scale structure, and statistics in the interpretation of positive-form adjectives. In T. Snider (Ed.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (Vol. 23, pp. 587–610). Ithaca, NY: CLC.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lassiter, D., & Goodman, N. D. (2017). Adjectival vagueness in a Bayesian model of interpretation. 194(10), 3801–3836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leffel, T., Xiang, M., & Kennedy, C. (2016). Imprecision is pragmatic: Evidence from referential processing. In M. Moroney, C. R. Little, J. Collard, & D. Burgdorf (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (Vol. 26). Publisher: Linguistic Society of America and Cornell Linguistics. CircleUrl: https://journals.linguisticsociety.org/proceedings/index.php/SALT/article/view/26.836/3688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maes, A., Arts, A., & Noordman, L. (2004). Reference management in instructive discourse. Discourse Process, 37(2), 117–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, L. (2011). The relative role of property type and scale structure in explaining the bahavior of gradable adjectives. In R. Nouwen, R. van Rooij, U. Sauerland, & H.-C. Schmitz (Eds.), Vagueness in communication. Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Vol. 6517, pp. 151–168). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking contraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13(4), 329–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paraboni, I., van Deemter, K., & Masthoff, J. (2007). Generating referring expressions: Making referents easy to identify. Computational Linguistics, 33(2), 229–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pechmann, T. (1989). Incremental speech production and referential overspecification. Linguistics, 27(1), 89–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinkal, M. (1995). Logic and lexicon: The semantics of the indefinite. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyykkönen-Klauck, P., & Crocker, M. W. (2016). Attention and eye movement metrics in visual world eye tracking. In P. Knoeferle, P. Pyykkönen-Klauck, & M. W. Crocker (Eds.), Visually situated language comprehension (pp. 67–82). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Qing, C., & Franke, M. (2014). Gradable adjectives, vagueness, and optimal language use: A speaker-oriented model. In T. Snider, S. D’Antonio, & M. Weigand (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (Vol. 24, pp. 23–41). Ithaca, NY: CLC.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothschild, D., & Segal, G. (2009). Indexical predicates. Mind and Language, 24(4), 467–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotstein, C., & Winter, Y. (2004). Total adjectives vs. partial adjectives: Scale structure and higher-order modifiers. Natural Language Semantics, 12(3), 259–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubio-Fernández, P. (2015). Redundancy is efficient–and effective, too. Paper presented at the XI Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubio-Fernández, P. (2016). How redundant are redundant color adjectives? An efficiency-based analysis of color overspecification. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(153).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sassoon, G. W., & Toledo, A. (2011). Absolute and relative adjectives and their comparison classes. Unpublished manuscript. Amsterdam university and Utrecht university.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedivy, J. C. (2003). Pragmatic versus form-based accounts of referential contrast: Evidence for effects of informativity expectations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedivy, J. C. (2004). Evaluating explanations for referential context effects: Evidence for Gricean meachanisms in online language interpretation. In J. C. Trueswell & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions (pp. 345–364). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71(2), 109–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Syrett, K., Kennedy, C., & Lidz, J. (2009). Meaning and context in children’s understanding of gradable adjectives. Journal of Semantics, 27(1), 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information during spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unger, P. (1975). Ignorance: A case for scepticism. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Rooij, R. (2011). Vagueness and linguistics. In G. Ronzitti (Ed.), Vagueness: A guide (Chap. 6, pp. 123–179). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). Finding referencts in timr: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49(3), 367–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westerbeek, H., Koolen, R., & Maes, A. (2015). Stored object knowledge and the production of referring expressions: The case of color typicality. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(935).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolter, L., Skovbroten Gorman, K., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). Scalar reference, contrast and discourse: Separating effects of linguistic discourse from availability of the referent. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(3), 299–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Aaron Hill, Jackson Lee, Gabriel Aparicio and Katie Franich for their assistance during various stages of the eye-tracking experiment reported in this paper, as well as Michelle Namkoong and Sonia Juan Rubio for help in the creation of part of the visual stimuli. Finally, we would also like to thank the audiences at the workshop on Gradability, Scale Structure, and Vagueness: Experimental Perspectives, and the 22nd AMLaP Conference. This project was supported by a NSF grant (BCS 1227144) to C. Kennedy and M. Xiang.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helena Aparicio .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

1 Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (zip 38388 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Aparicio, H., Kennedy, C., Xiang, M. (2018). Perceived Informativity and Referential Effects of Contrast in Adjectivally Modified NPs. In: Castroviejo, E., McNally, L., Weidman Sassoon, G. (eds) The Semantics of Gradability, Vagueness, and Scale Structure. Language, Cognition, and Mind, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77791-7_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77791-7_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-77790-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-77791-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics