Skip to main content

Debonding and Clipping of Prefixoids in Germanic: Constructionalization or Constructional Change?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Construction of Words

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 4))

Abstract

This paper is concerned with the debonding of three Germanic prefixoids: Dutch kei ‘boulder’, German Hammer ‘hammer’, and Swedish kanon ‘cannon’. Drawing on an extensive corpus-based and statistical analysis, we compare the formal properties (construction types), semantics (degree of bleaching), collocational properties and productivity of bound and free uses of each prefixoid. We show that debonding of prefixoids is a productive process of lexical innovation in Germanic languages, which may lead to the creation of new intensifying adverbs or evaluative adjectives. In addition, we explore whether debonding of prefixoids can be fruitfully analysed from a constructional perspective. More in particular, we address the question of whether the observed changes accompanying debonding are best accounted for by Traugott and Trousdale’s concept of ‘constructionalization’, or by Hilpert’s concept of ‘constructional change’. To this end, we explore a variety of quantitative methods, including productivity measures and distinctive collexeme analysis. We conclude that the quantitative differences between the bound and the free forms of the three prefixoids studied in this paper allow us to consider them as two separate constructions, but that the distinction is a gradient one.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    There is some controversy regarding the morphological status of prefixoids. Although it is generally acknowledged that they are semantically different from the free morphemes they derive from and may have specific formal properties, several authors have argued that this does not imply that they form a distinct type of morpheme. This issue is outside of the scope of this paper – for discussion, see Norde and Van Goethem (2015); Norde and Morris (2018) or Battefeld et al. (2018), and references therein.

  2. 2.

    Prefixoids with intensifying function in [N-ADJ] compounds are found in all Germanic languages except English. English did borrow über- from German (übercool, übersexy; Van der Wouden and Foolen 2017: 85), but this is not a prefixoid in the strict sense because it does not correspond to a free English lexeme.

  3. 3.

    On the development of affixoids see further, among others, Stevens (2005); Pittner and Berman (2006); Berman (2009); Leuschner (2010); Hoeksema (2012); Klara (2012); Meibauer (2013); Hüning and Booij (2014); Battefeld et al. (2018).

  4. 4.

    Thanks to Sarah Sippach for drawing our attention to German end, and finding corpus examples.

  5. 5.

    German adjectival inflection features three genders, four cases, as well as a contrast between definite and indefinite forms in the singular; and 4 cases and definite/indefinite contrast in the plural. This makes 32 contexts, although many of these forms have the same suffix.

  6. 6.

    Thanks to Roland Pooth for providing us with example (17).

  7. 7.

    Neoanalysis is a term taken from Andersen (2001) and refers to the creation of a “new representation in the mind of a language user” (Traugott and Trousdale 2013: 21), which is argued to be “a micro-step in a constructional change” (p. 36).

  8. 8.

    As the parameters of schematicity and compositionality, as defined by Traugott and Trousdale (2013), are difficult to operationalize in our case studies, we will not use them in the remainder of this article.

  9. 9.

    The corpus is available, after registration, at https://www.webcorpora.org//

  10. 10.

    We quote literally from the corpora, which means that spelling errors have not been edited.

  11. 11.

    Each single token had to be analysed separately, because some tokens had to be discarded, whereas other were relevant to this study. For example, Hammerfilm could mean ‘a movie from the Hammer House of Horror studios’ or ‘a great movie’. Tokens with the former meaning were removed from the data set.

  12. 12.

    Of the first 200 tokens in the sample, only 51 were relevant to our study. This would imply that we would have needed to sift through 4000 tokens (manually) to obtain a 1000 token sample including upper case Hammer.

  13. 13.

    This finding supports Hoeksema’s (2012) account, according to which similes (“compounds expressing stereotyped comparisons”) and compounds beginning with an intensifying prefixoid (“analogical extensions of comparison-based compounds”) belong to the same class of “elative compounds”, and may undergo emphatic reduplicative conjunction in a similar way as regular adverbs of degree (e.g. ijs- en ijskoud ‘ice and ice cold; extremely cold’, erg maar dan ook erg koud ‘very but indeed very cold; really very cold’, zeer en zeer koud ‘very and very cold’) (Hoeksema 2012: 98–99). Since this emphatic construction is available for both intensifying compounds and adverbs, it is not a conclusive criterion to range these uses of kei as instances of either an intensifying adverb or an orthographically separated prefixoid.

  14. 14.

    The examples are from Deutsches Textarchiv (http://www.deutschestextarchiv.de)

  15. 15.

    http://www.duden.de/sprachwissen/rechtschreibregeln/bindestrich#K26

  16. 16.

    They do not use the term prefixoid, however, but speak of an “Adjektivkompositum mit intensivierender Bedeutung”.

  17. 17.

    Note also that, even in simile constructions such as (75), an intensifying reading is not precluded – hammerharte Bässe can also mean ‘very cool basses’ (Lars Erik Zeige, p.c.).

  18. 18.

    The sample does not contain examples of emphatic reduplication (compare the kei examples (43–46) above).

  19. 19.

    An earlier example (1889) is however found in a historical corpus in Språkbanken (https://spraakbanken.gu.se/). Thanks to Henrik Rosenkvist for finding this example.

  20. 20.

    This is a common association in other languages as well, compare French bourré comme un canon, German voll wie eine Kanone, Dutch zo dronken als een kanon.

  21. 21.

    Unlike German and Dutch, Swedish (marginally) allows bound prefixoids with verbs (Ascoop and Leuschner 2006: 246), but these do not occur in our sample. A Google search yields few examples (e.g. vi kanontrivdes ‘we enjoyed ourselves tremendously’ (thailandforum.se › sp)).

  22. 22.

    Other debonded prefixes or prefixoids, e.g. super or skit ‘shit’, do not inflect either. Moreover, there are a few indeclinable Swedish adjectives, e.g. bra ‘good, fine’, or kul ‘cool’. Interestingly, these adjectives can be used as exclamatives as well, just like kanon, super and skit, so it may well be that kanon will continue to pattern with these adjectives and not acquire inflection.

  23. 23.

    Another example of a free prexoid occurring in the genitive plural is kalasers, from the intensifying prefixoid kalas-, originally a noun meaning ‘party’ (Ledin 2012).

  24. 24.

    Generally, kanoners and free kanon can be used in the same constructions, with the same meaning (kanoners/kanon bra ‘very good’, kanoners/kanon dag ‘great day’ etc.), but kanoners is far less frequent in SECOW14AX (1400 raw hits) than kanon (more than 10,000 hits, which is the maximum number of results in Colibri2 queries).

  25. 25.

    The frequencies per million tokens for the lower case forms are: bra (indeclinable): 2138.973; fin/fint/fina: 823.2269; god/gott/goda: 681.9374; kul (indeclinable): 460.0758. The frequencies can be found at http://hpsg.fu-berlin.de/cow/frequencies/swedish/

  26. 26.

    The effect size is given as Cramér’s V, which indicates correlation strength: 0.10–0.30 indicates a small effect size; 0.30 to 0.50 a moderate one, and >0.50 a large one. We used the vcd package for R (Meyer et al. 2016) to compute it.

  27. 27.

    In the raw corpus data (10,000 hits, the maximum), there is only one single example of the simile construction kanonhård ‘cannon hard’, so this particular collocation does not appear to be very productive.

  28. 28.

    Note that we do not have the data for the entire corpus, but use the frequencies in the samples instead. Therefore, these statistics can only be used for comparison of the bound and free forms in the sample, not of those in the corpus as a whole.

  29. 29.

    This spelling variant of jätte can be explained as due to the tendency in Modern Swedish to write compounds as two words (Teleman et al. 1999: 57).

References

  • Andersen, H. 2001. Actualization and the (uni)directionality of change. In Actualization. Linguistic change in progress, ed. H. Andersen, 225–248. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ascoop, K., and T. Leuschner. 2006. “Affixoidhungrig? Skitbra!” Comparing affixoids in German and Swedish. Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung 59 (3): 241–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, H. 2009. Corpus linguistics in morphology: Morphological productivity. In Corpus Linguistics. An international handbook, ed. A. Lüdeling and M. Kytö, 900–919. Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J., E. Smirnova, L. Sommerer, and S. Gildea. 2015. Diachronic Construction Grammar. John Benjamins: Amsterdam/Philadelphia.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Battefeld, M., T. Leuschner, and G. Rawoens. 2018. Evaluative morphology’ in German, Dutch and Swedish: Constructional networks and the loci of change. In Category change from a constructional perspective, ed. K. Van Goethem, M. Norde, E. Coussé, and G. Vanderbauwhede, 229–262. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bergs, A., and G. Diewald, eds. 2008. Constructions and language change (Trends in linguistics. Studies and monographs 194). Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———, eds. 2009. Contexts and constructions (Constructional Approaches to Language 9). John Benjamins: Amsterdam/Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, J. 2009. The predicative as a source of grammatical variation. In Describing and modeling variation in grammar, ed. A. Dufter, J. Fleischer, and G. Seiler, 99–116. Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. 2009. Compounding and Construction Morphology. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. R. Lieber and P. Štekauer, 201–216. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Smet, H. 2012. The course of actualization. Language 88: 601–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gries, S.T., and A. Stefanowitsch. 2004. Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on ‘alternations’. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 9 (1): 97–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilpert, M. 2013. Constructional change in English. Developments in allomorphy, word formation and syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. From hand-carved to computer-based: Noun-participle compounding and the upward strengthening hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics 26 (1): 113–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoeksema, J. 2012. Elative compounds in Dutch: Properties and developments. In Intensivierungskonzepte bei Adjektiven und Adverben im Sprachenvergleich/Crosslinguistic comparison of intensified adjectives and adverbs, ed. G. Oebel, 97–142. Hamburg: Kovač Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hüning, M., and G. Booij. 2014. From compounding to derivation. The emergence of derivational affixes through “constructionalization”. Folia Linguistica 48 (2): 579–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klara, L. 2012. Steinreich ist saucool: Adjektivische Steigerungsbildungen des Gegenwartsdeutschen. In Intensivierungskonzepte bei Adjektiven und Adverben im Sprachvergleich, ed. G. Oebel, 143–170. Hamburg: Dr. Kovač.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledin, P. 2012. Positivens interjektioner: grymmerst! finerst! kanoners och kalasers! braish! På svenska 24.5.2012. https://pasvenska.se/positivismens-interjektioner-grymmerst-finerst-kanoners-och-kalasers-braish/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leuschner, T. 2010. Ausnahmepianist fettgeschreckt – inbleich! Deutsche, niederländische und schwedische Präfixoide im Spannungsfeld von Genealogie, Kreativität und Norm. In Kontrastive Germanistische Linguistik, ed. A. Dammel, S. Kürschner, and D. Nübling, 863–892. Hildesheim: Olms.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levshina, N. 2014. Rling: A companion package for how to do linguistics with R. R package version 1.0.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. How to do linguistics with R. Data exploration and statistical analysis. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lundbladh, C.-E. 2002. Prefixlika förleder. In In Det Svenska ordförrådets utveckling 1800–2000. Göteborg: Institutionen för svenska språket. http://spraakdata.gu.se/ordat/pdf/Ordat17utsida.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meibauer, J. 2013. Expressive compounds in German. Word Structure 6: 21–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D., A. Zeileis, and K. Hornik. 2016. vcd: Visualizing categorical data. R package version 1.4–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noël, D. 2016. For a radically usage-based diachronic construction grammar. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 30: 39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norde, M. 2009. Degrammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Norde, M., and C. Morris. 2018. Derivation without category change: A network-based analysis of diminutive prefixoids in Dutch. In Category change from a constructional perspective, ed. K. Van Goethem, M. Norde, E. Coussé, and G. Vanderbauwhede, 47–90. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Norde, M., and K. Van Goethem. 2014. Bleaching, productivity and debonding of prefixoids. A corpus-based analysis of ‘giant’ in German and Swedish. Lingvisticae Investigationes 37 (2): 256–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. Emancipatie van affixen en affixoïden: Degrammaticalisatie of lexicalisatie? Nederlandse Taalkunde 20 (1): 109–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittner, K., and J. Berman. 2006. Video ist echt schrott aber single ist hammer. Jugendsprachliche Nomen-Adjektiv-Konversion in der Prädikativposition. Deutsche Sprache 3: 233–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. 2016. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/.

  • Schäfer, R. 2015. Processing and querying large web corpora with the COW 14 architecture. In Proceedings of Challenges in the Management of Large Corpora (CMLC-3, July 20, 2015, Lancaster).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, R., and F. Bildhauer. 2012. Building large corpora from the web using a new efficient tool chain. In Proceedings of the eight international conference on language resources and evaluation (LREC’12), ed. N. Calzolari et al., 486–493. Istanbul: ELRA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlücker, B. 2013. Non-classifying compounds in German. Folia Linguistica 47 (2): 449–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, C.M. 2005. Revisiting the affixoid debate: On the grammaticalization of the word. In Grammatikalisierung im Deutschen, ed. T. Leuschner, T. Mortelmans, and S. De Groodt, 71–83. Berlin/New York: Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teleman, U., S. Hellberg, and E. Andersson. 1999. Svenska Akademiens Grammatik.II: Ord. Stockholm: Svenska Akademien.

    Google Scholar 

  • Traugott, E.C., and G. Trousdale. 2013. Constructionalization and constructional changes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Trousdale, G. 2008. A constructional approach to lexicalization processes in the history of English: Evidence from possessive constructions. Word Structure 1: 156–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. Multiple inheritance and constructional change. Studies in Language 37 (3): 491–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trousdale, G., and M. Norde. 2013. Degrammaticalization and constructionalization: Two case studies. Language Sciences 36: 32–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Velde, F., H. De Smet, and L. Ghesquière. 2013. On multiple source constructions in language change. Studies in Language 37 (3): 473–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Sijs, N. (Ed.) 2010. Etymologiebank, http://etymologiebank.nl/.

  • Van der Wouden, T., and A. Foolen. 2017. A most serious and extraordinary problem. Intensification of adjectives in Dutch, German, and English. Leuvense Bijdragen 101: 82–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Goethem, K., and H. De Smet. 2014. How nouns turn into adjectives. The emergence of new adjectives in French, English and Dutch through debonding processes. Languages in Contrast 14 (2): 251–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Goethem, K., and Ph. Hiligsmann. 2014. When two paths converge: debonding and clipping of Dutch reuze ‘lit. giant; great’. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 26 (1): 31–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Goethem, K., and M. Hüning. 2015. From noun to evaluative adjective: Conversion or debonding? Dutch top and its equivalents in German. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 27 (4): 366–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Goethem, K., and N. Koutsoukos. forthcoming. ‘Morphological transposition’ as the onset of recategorization. The case of luxe in Dutch. Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Goethem, K., M. Norde, E. Coussé, and G. Vanderbauwhede, eds. 2018. Category change from a constructional perspective. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

Corpora:

Dictionaries:

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Muriel Norde .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Norde, M., Van Goethem, K. (2018). Debonding and Clipping of Prefixoids in Germanic: Constructionalization or Constructional Change?. In: Booij, G. (eds) The Construction of Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_17

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74393-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74394-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics