Skip to main content

Japanese Word Formation in Construction Morphology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 1074 Accesses

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 4))

Abstract

Along with the growing number of studies taking a construction approach to phrasal and clausal phenomena, applications of Construction Grammar to morphology has highlighted its importance in analyzing human language. In this chapter, we will illustrate that the construction morphology approach is most insightful in analyzing four word formation phenomena in Japanese: innovative verbs, hypocoristic formation, intensified mimetic adverbs, and innovative prenominal noun modification. They exhibit that a set of properties jointly belong to the construction in which they appear. These properties individually or collectively do not follow from general or Japanese-specific morpho(phono)logical principles. In each case, the properties that pertain to its form, meaning, and usage are better captured holistically belonging to the construction itself. The construction morphology approach offers the conceptual framework and methodological tools for analyzing them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Innovative verbs can also be formed around mimetic words, whose categorial status is often indeterminate. Examples include nikoru ‘smile’, chibiru ‘stint’, pakuru ‘swindle’, and guzuru ‘grizzle’. In this chapter we will focus on innovative verbs that are denominal although the analysis presented here applies to mimetic-based innovative verbs as well. For more details, see Tsujimura and Davis (2011).

  2. 2.

    Higashikokubaru has an additional meaning, ‘to show up no matter how busy he is or even when he is not invited.’ This meaning is supposed to reflect Governor Higashikokubaru’s personal traits. It is of note that a trip to Miyazaki and an uninvited appearance may not inherently share anything common but both emerge as actual usages because they mirror some aspect related to Higashikokubaru.

  3. 3.

    The construction schema in (6) may not seem to account for sutabaru, which is paradigmatically related to sutaabakkusu in (1), because the long vowel /aa/ is shortened in the verb form. Another example is found in guuguruguguru ‘to conduct a Google search’ and jakuujii ‘jacuzzi’ ↔ jakujiru ‘to use jacuzzi’. As we noted in Tsujimura and Davis (2011: 804), this reflects a tendency that the last two moras of an innovative verb root does not consist of a long vowel. This tendency may lead to a modification that the last C in the […[(C)V(C)V’]k-r]Vroot portion of the schema should always surface rather than be optional.

  4. 4.

    The suffix –tyan appears in kinship terms like (o-)too-tyan ‘father’, (o-)kaa-tyan ‘mother’, (o-)nii-tyan ‘older brother’, (o-)nee-tyan ‘older sister’, (o-)jii-tyan ‘grandfather’, (o-)baa-tyan ‘grandmother’, o-ji-tyan ‘uncle’, and o-ba-tyan ‘aunt’ although these forms have corresponding terms with –san instead of –tyan. Additionally, there are words like bot-tyan ‘small boy, someone’s son’, ojoo-tyan ‘small girl’, and aka-tyan ‘baby’, also sharing the same suffix. However, these words are different from truncated hypocoristics in Japanese to be discussed in this section in that they do not involve a truncation process.

  5. 5.

    When first names consist of two moras, truncation does not take place, yielding hypocoristic counterparts with –tyan, as in Mari ➔ mari-tyan, Rika ➔ rika-tyan, and Jun ➔ jun-tyan. The degree of endearment in these hypocoristics in comparison with truncated longer names is not clear to us. This is similar to the observation made by Booij and Audring (2017) that monosyllabic English names that do not have a correspondence to a longer form (e.g. Paul) lack a sense of endearment.

  6. 6.

    The only exceptions that we are aware of are cases in which the truncation results in three moras that contain a geminate consonant. Examples include Akiko ➔ akko-tyan, Tokuko ➔ tokko-tyan, Yoshiko ➔ okko-tyan, and Motoko ➔ mokko-tyan. As these examples suggest, many of the original first names of three-mora hypocoristics have high vowels /i, u/ between voiceless consonants, resulting in their devoicing. The environment for high vowel devoicing seems to correspond to the geminate consonant in these examples. As for cases like Motoko ➔ mokko-tyan, while the full name does not have any high vowel, Vance (2008) notes that non-high vowels often become devoiced between voiceless consonants although it happens much less consistently than with high vowels. In light of the fact that the second vowel of Motoko meets the environment of vowel devoicing, the three-mora hypocoristic form with a geminate consonant may be seen as phonologically derived from the suffixing of –tyan to the full name.

  7. 7.

    A consonant serving as the second mora of a truncated hypocoristic is always realized uniformly as homorganic to the following consonant, but this constraint does not need to be stipulated in the formal representation of the construction because non-homorganic consonant clusters are not allowed in Japanese.

  8. 8.

    Possible exceptions exist but mostly in very casual speech. For instance, [nn] (in HL pitch) [nnn] [in LHL pitch] can be used for ‘yes’ and ‘no’ respectively; and ppoi ‘like [X]’ and tte iu ka ‘or should I say’, are considered elliptical, assuming that full nouns and statements precede the initial geminates.

  9. 9.

    As Poser (1990) observes, these hypocoristics may have varying degrees of acceptability among different speakers.

  10. 10.

    Hamano (1998) further analyzes /r/ to “symbolize[s] smooth movement” while the vowel /i/ “seems epenthetic without any symbolic correlate” (p. 107). In Akita (2009) /-ri/ suffixed to a CVCV mimetic base where the second V is accented “connotes quietness” (p. 175).

  11. 11.

    For the optionality of the quotative particle –to, see Akita and Usuki (2016) and Toratani (2006, 2017).

  12. 12.

    The same observations are found in Tamori and Schourup (1999) and Moriyama (2002), among others.

  13. 13.

    The concept of mora augmentation may be originated from Kuroda (1979), who called C0 “a mora consonant.”

  14. 14.

    See Audring and Booij (2016) for the role of coercion effects in Construction Morphology.

  15. 15.

    In a similar vein, the second example in (22c), otona-na machi, has a narrow interpretation: it refers to Ginza as a shopping area filled with exclusive, high-end stores. The “property” reading of otona ‘adult’ in this case seems quite stretched from the conventional denotation of the noun otona, but the exact nature of the connection between an aspect of otona in the context of Ginza as a shopping site is left up to language users to determine. That is, such flexibility and subjectivity of interpretations of the prenominal N modifiers is part of the N-na N construction that distinguishes it from the N-no N pattern.

  16. 16.

    Other examples like those in (22) include suupaa-hiiroo-na sekai (super-hero-na world), toogarashi-na aitsu (red pepper-na guy), and enka-na aitsu (enka [traditional-style Japanese popular songs]-na guy), among others, each of which calls for a specific interpretation that is appropriate for a particular context and situation, although it is possible to assign a more general “property” interpretation.

References

  • Akita, K. 2009. A grammar of sound-symbolic words in Japanese: Theoretical approaches to iconic and lexical properties of mimetics. Ph.D. dissertation, Kobe University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akita, K., and T. Usuki. 2016. A constructional account of the ‘optional’ quotative marking on Japanese mimetics. Journal of Linguistics 52: 245–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. 1976. Word formation and generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1980. Contextuals. Language 56: 744–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Audring, J., and G. Booij. 2016. Cooperation and coercion. Linguistics 54: 617–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. 2005. Compounding and derivation: Evidence for construction morphology. In Morphology and its demarcations, ed. W.U. Dressler, D. Kastovsky, O. Pfeiffer, and F. Rainer, 109–132. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2007. Construction morphology and the lexicon. In Selected proceedings of the 5th décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse, ed. F. Montermini, G. Boyé, and N. Hathout, 34–44. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2009a. Constructions and lexical units: An analysis of Dutch numerals. Linguistische Berichte. Sonderheft 19: 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2009b. Compounding and construction morphology. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. R. Lieber and P. Štekauer, 201–216. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G., and J. Audring. 2017. Construction morphology and the parallel architecture of grammar. Cognitive Science 41 (S2): 277–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E., and H. Clark. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55: 767–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. 2001. Radical construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S., and N. Tsujimura. 2018. Arabic nonconcatenative morphology in construction morphology. In The construction of words, Advances in construction morphology, 315–339. Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S., and I. Ueda. 2002. Mora augmentation processes in Japanese. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 18: 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H.P. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Syntax and semantics III: Speech act, ed. P. Cole and J.L. Morgan, 41–58. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamano, S. 1998. The sound-symbolic system of Japanese. Stanford/Tokyo: CSLI Publications/Kurosio Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, J. 1990. Prosodic minimality in Japanese. Chicago Linguistic Society 26: 213–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwasaki, S. 2002. Japanese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kageyama, T. 1982. Word formation in Japanese. Lingua 57: 215–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamei, H. 2003. Wakamonokotobajiten. [Dictionary of youth language] Tokyo: Nihon Hoosoo Shuppan Kyookai.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kato, C. 2005. Wakamonokotobajiten. [Dictionary of youth language] Nagoya: Chuubu Nihon Kyooiku Bunkakai.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koyano, T. 1993. Joshidaisei yoogo no kiso chishiki [Basic knowledge of female college students’ vocabulary] Osaka: Osaka University of Foreign Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubozono, H. 1999. Mora and syllable. In The handbook of Japanese linguistics, ed. N. Tsujimura, 31–61. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1979. Generative grammatical studies in the Japanese language. New York: Garland Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mester, A., and J. Ito. 1989. Feature predictability and underspecification: Palatal prosody in Japanese mimetics. Language 65: 258–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaelis, L. 2003. Word meaning, sentence meaning and construction meaning. In Cognitive perspectives on lexical semantics, ed. H. Cuyckens, R. Dirven, and J. Taylor, 163–210. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaelis, L., and K. Lambrecht. 1996. Toward a construction-based theory of language functions: The case of nominal extraposition. Language 72: 215–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyagawa, S. 1987. Lexical categories in Japanese. Lingua 81: 29–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moriyama, K. 2002. “CVQCVri”-gata no onomatope. [On mimetics of the “CVQCVri”-pattern] Journal of International Student Center 10: 53–71. Nagasaki University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poser, W.J. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66: 78–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, K. 2009. The noun-na noun construction as a marker of communality and interpersonal engagement. Paper presented at the 19th Japanese/Korean linguistics conference. University of Hawai’i at Manoa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamori, I., and L. Schourup. 1999. Onomatope: Keitai to imi. [Onomatopoeia: Form and meaning]. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toratani, K. 2006. On the optionality of to-marking on reduplicated mimetics in Japanese. In Japanese/Korean Linguistics, vol. 14, 415–422. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017. The position of to/Ø-marked mimetics in Japanese sentence structure. In The grammar of Japanese mimetics: Perspectives from structure, acquisition, and translation, ed. N. Iwasaki, P. Sells, and K. Akita, 35–72. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsujimura, N. 2010. On the pattern of innovative verbs. Paper presented at the workshop on the lexicon. The National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics, Tokyo.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2014. An introduction to Japanese linguistics. 3rd ed. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsujimura, N., and S. Davis. 2008a. A construction approach to innovative verbs in Japanese. Paper presented at the 5th international conference on construction grammar. University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008b. Innovative morphology: A case of verbal conjugation in Japanese. Paper presented at the 6th international conference on practical linguistics of Japanese. San Francisco State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Japanese innovative verbs as a morphological construction. Paper presented at the 14th International Morphology Meeting. Budapest.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2011. A construction approach to innovative verbs in Japanese. Cognitive Linguistics 22: 797–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uehara, S. 1998. Syntactic categories in Japanese: A cognitive and typological introduction. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vance, T. 2008. The sounds of Japanese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, N. 2007. Wakamonokotoba ni mimi o sumaseba. [If you listen to youth language] Tokyo: Kodansha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yonekawa, A. 1989. Shingo to ryuukoogo [New words and trendy words] Tokyo: Nanundo.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank Geert Booij not only for inviting us to contribute to this volume but also for providing us with a number of insightful comments. We are particularly indebted to him for his guidance to improve the content of this article and to help us to better understand the specific mechanisms of Construction Morphology. We also thank an anonymous reviewer for useful comments on an earlier version of this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Natsuko Tsujimura .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Tsujimura, N., Davis, S. (2018). Japanese Word Formation in Construction Morphology. In: Booij, G. (eds) The Construction of Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74393-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74394-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics