Skip to main content

Killing Time with Technology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
A Psychology of User Experience

Part of the book series: Human–Computer Interaction Series ((HCIS))

  • 2969 Accesses

Abstract

This book has presented a psychology of user experience and in doing so we have adopted a philosophical perspective to frame at least some of the ways in which we interact or use or otherwise employ digital products.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The term “autotelic” derives from two Greek words, auto meaning self, and telos meaning goal. It refers to a self-contained activity, one that is done not with the expectation of some future benefit, but simply because the doing itself is the reward.

  2. 2.

    The following section has been adapted from an interview in the Atlantic magazine. https://www.theatlantic.com/technology/archive/2017/06/the-app-that-does-nothing/529764/.

  3. 3.

    A final note on involvement. Heidegger tells us that care is the fundamental basis of our being-in-the-world and reveals itself in a variety of ways, for example: (and here we borrow freely from Wheeler 2011) as having to do something, producing something, attending to something and looking after it, making use of something, undertaking, accomplishing, considering, determining and so forth. In short, because we care, we are involved. However, we have chosen to base this account of UX on involvement rather than care, as the former is more manageable and familiar to the HCI professional for whom this book is written. However, we do recognise that a fuller treatment of human computer interaction is possible but would require care.

  4. 4.

    Heidegger’s usually untranslated word for “human-being”.

References

  • Bar M, Aminoff E, Mason M, Fenske M (2007) The units of thought. Hippocampus 17(6):420–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124(1):1–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crosby B, Bendix W, Hardwicke C (1949) Busy doing nothing. Music by Jimmy Van Heusen. Lyrics by Johnny Burke. Paramount Pictures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1992) Flow: the psychology of happiness. Rider, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1996) Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. Harper Collins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1997) Finding flow: the psychology of engagement with everyday life. Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (2014) Toward a psychology of optimal experience. In: Flow and the foundations of positive psychology. Springer Netherlands, pp 209–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M, Abuhamdeh S, Nakamura J (2014) Flow. In: Flow and the foundations of positive psychology. Springer Netherlands, pp 227–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey J (1934/1980) (reprint) Art as experience. Perigee Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassenzahl M (2008) User experience (UX): towards an experiential perspective on product quality. In: Proceedings of the 20th Conference on l’Interaction Homme-Machine. ACM, pp 11–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar DH (1974) Patterns of brain activity revealed by measurements of regional cerebral blood flow. Alfred Benzon Symposium VIII, Copenhagen

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar DH (1979) “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness: on the functional anatomy of the conscious state. Acta Neurol Scand 60:12–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar DH (1985) “Memory of the future”: an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. Hum Neurobiol 4:127–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D (2011) Fast Thinking, Slow Thinking

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger E (1971) Structure and functions of fantasy. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • McVay JC, Kane MJ (2010) Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychol Bull 136(2):188–207

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitcham C (1994) Thinking through Technology: the Path between Engineering and Philosophy. Chicago University Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Mooneyham BW, Schooler JW (2013) The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 67(1):11–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman DA (2004) Emotional design: why we love or hate everyday objects. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony A, Clore GL, Collins A (1988) The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge university press

    Google Scholar 

  • Pace-Schott EF (2013) Dreaming as a story-telling instinct. Front Psychol 4:159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98(2):676–682

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skeat WW (1910) An etymological dictionary of the English language. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiger L (1992) The pursuit of pleasure. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle S, Essig T, Russell GI (2017) Afterword: reclaiming psychoanalysis: Sherry Turkle in conversation with the editors. Psychoanal Perspect 14(2):237–248

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valera FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (1991) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler M (2011) “Martin Heidegger”, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2011 edn) In: Zalta EN (ed) http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2011/entries/heidegger. Last accessed 1 Sept 2017

Bibliography

  • Akah B, Bardzell S (2010) Empowering products: personal identity through the act of appropriation. In: CHI’10 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems. pp 4021–4026

    Google Scholar 

  • Aly A, Tapus A (2013, March) A model for synthesizing a combined verbal and nonverbal behavior based on personality traits in human-robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the 8th ACM/IEEE international conference on Human-robot interaction. IEEE Press, pp 325–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews S, Ellis DA, Shaw H, Piwek L (2015) Beyond self-report: tools to compare estimated and real-world smartphone use. PloS One 10(10):e0139004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Augustin MD, Wagemans J, Carbon CC (2012) All is beautiful? Generality versus specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychologica 139:1187–1201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson JN, Beall AC, Blascovich J, Raimundo M, Weisbuch M (2001) Intelligent agents who wear your face: users’ reactions to the virtual self. Int Workshop Intell Virtual Agents. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg, pp 86–99

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baird B, Smallwood J, Mrazek MD, Kam JWY, Franklin MS, Schooler JW (2012) Inspired by distraction: mind-wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychol Sci 23(10):1117–1122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S (2004) The essential difference. Penguin UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1987) The instability of graded structure: implications for the nature of concepts. In: Neiser U (ed) Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 101–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2003a). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philos Trans R Soc London B Biol Sci, 358(1435):1177–1187

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (2003b) Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Lang Cogn Process 18:513–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartholow BD, Anderson CA (2002) Effects of violent digital games on aggressive behavior: potential sex differences. J Exp Soc Psychol 38(3):283–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholow BD, Sestir MA, Davis EB (2005) Correlates and consequences of exposure to digital game violence: hostile personality, empathy, and aggressive behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 31(11):1573–1586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholow BD, Bushman BJ, Sestir MA (2006) Chronic violent digital game exposure and desensitization to violence: behavioral and event-related brain potential data. J Exp Soc Psychol 42(4):532–539

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaty RE, Silvia PJ, Nusbaum EC, Jauk E, Benedek M (2014) The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Mem Cogn 42(7):1186–1197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Bassat T, Meyer J, Tractinsky N (2006) Economic and subjective measures of the perceived value of aesthetics and usability. ACM Trans Comp Hum Inter 13(2):210–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson JE, Sabbagh MA, Carlson SM, Zelazo PD (2013) Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Dev Psychol 49(9):1615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickmore TW, Caruso L, Clough-Gorr K, Heeren T (2005) ‘It’s just like you talk to a friend’ relational agents for older adults. Interact Comp 17(6):711–735

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biswal B, Zerrin Yetkin F, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echoplanar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34(4):537–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blevis E, Stolterman E (2007) Ensoulment and sustainable interaction design. In: Proceedings of the International Association of Societies of Design Research. The Honk Kong Polytechnic University, 12th–15th November 2007

    Google Scholar 

  • Blythe MA, Hassenzahl M, Law E, Vermeeren A (2007) An analysis framework for user experience (UX) studies: a green paper. Towards a UX Manifesto, p 1

    Google Scholar 

  • Bødker S (1991) Through the interface—a human activity approach to user interface design. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Bødker S, Lyle P, Saad-Sulonen J (2017) Untangling the mess of technological artifacts: investigating community artifact ecologies. In: Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Communities and Technologies. ACM, pp 246–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehner K, Sengers P, Warner S (2008) Interfaces with the ineffable: meeting aesthetic experience on its own terms. ACM Trans Comp Hum Interact 15(3):12

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgmann A (1984) Technology and the character of contemporary life. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgmann A (2000) Reply to My Critics. In: Higgs E, Light A, Strong D (eds) Technology and the good life?. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein RF (1992) Subliminal mere exposure effects. In: Bornstein RF, Pittman TS (eds) Perception without awareness. Guilford Press, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockmyer JH, Fox CM, Curtiss KA, McBroom E, Burkhart KM, Pidruzny JN (2009) The development of the game engagement questionnaire: a measure of engagement in video game-playing. J Exp Soc Psychol 45(4):624–634

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broekens J, Heerink M, Rosendal H (2009) Assistive social robots in elderly care: a review. Gerontechnology 8(2):94–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cafaro A, Vilhjálmsson HH, Bickmore T, Heylen D, Jóhannsdóttir KR, Valgarðsson GS (2012, September) First impressions: users’ judgments of virtual agents’ personality and interpersonal attitude in first encounters. In: International Conference on Intelligent Virtual Agents. Springer Berlin Heidelberg, pp 67–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Card SK, Moran TP, Newell A (1983) The psychology of human-computer interaction. Hillsdale, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Cech CJ, Shoben EJ, Love M (1990) Multiple congruity effects in judgments of magnitude. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 16:1142–1152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cesta A, Cortellessa G, Giuliani V, Pecora F, Scopelliti M, Tiberio L (2007) Psychological implications of domestic assistive technology for the elderly. PsychNol J 5(3):229–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee A (2014) Scientific aesthetics: three steps forward. Br J Psychol 105(4):465–467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff K, Ream JM, Gabrieli JD (2004) Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 40:623–630

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PM (1991) Folk psychology and the explanation of human behavior. In: Greenwood JD (ed) The future of folk psychology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 51–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PM (1992) Matter and consciousness. MIT Press, Cambridge, AM

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinzia DD, Vittorio G (2009) Neuroaesthetics: a review. Curr Opin Neurobiol 19(6):682–687

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (2009) Spreading the joy? why the machinery of consciousness is (probably) still in the head. Mind 118(472):963–993

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A, Chalmers DJ (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58:10–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clavel C, Caroline F, Martin JC, Pesty S, Duhaut D (2013) Artificial companions with personality and social role. In: CICAC 2013—Symposium on Computational Intelligence for Creativity and Affective Computing, Singapore. IEEE, pp 87–95

    Google Scholar 

  • Clavel C, Faur C, Martin J-C, Pesty AS, Duhaut D (2013) Artificial companions with personality and social role. In: CICAC 2013—Symposium on Computational Intelligence for Creativity and Affective Computing, Apr 2013, Singapore. IEEE, pp 87–95, 2013, Symposium Series on Computational Intelligence (SSCI)

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2005) Appraising valence. In: Colombetti G, Thompson E (eds) Emotion experience. Imprint Academic, Thorverton, UK, pp 103–126. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 12:103–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2007a) Enactive appraisal. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 6:527–546

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2007b) Enactive appraisal. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 6(4):527–546

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2010a) Enaction, sense-making and emotion. In: Stewart J, Gapenne O, Di Paolo E (eds) Enaction: towards a new paradigm for cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, pp 145–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2010b) Enaction, sense-making and emotion. sToward a new paradigm for cognitive science, Enaction, pp 145–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti G (2014) The feeling body: affective science meets the enactive mind. MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway MA (2001) Sensory-perceptual episodic memory. Philos Trans R Soc B 356:1375–1384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (2000) Consider the source: the evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1990) Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. Harper and Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cupchik GC, Gebotys RJ (1988) The search for meaning in art: interpretive styles and judgments of quality. Vis Arts Res, 38–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmet PMA (2011) Nine sources of product emotion. In: Proceedings of the Conference: Interfejs użytkownika—Kansei w praktyce, Warszawa, pp 8–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmet PMA, Dijkhuis E (2003) A wheelchair can be fun: a case of emotion-derived design. In: Proceedings of DPPI’03. ACM Press, pp 22–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickey MD (2005) Engaging by design: how engagement strategies in popular computer and digital games can inform instructional design. Educ Technol Res Dev 53(2):67–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A, Meurs T (2006) Where creativity resides: the generative power of unconscious thought. Conscious Cogn 15:135–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSalvo C, Sengers P, Brynjarsdóttir H (2010, April) Mapping the landscape of sustainable HCI. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, pp 1975–1984

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar A, Dardar L (2015) Personal ecologies of calendar artefacts. J Interact Sci 3(2). Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40166-015-0007-x

  • Djamasbi S, Siegel M, Skorinko J, Tullis T (2011) Online viewing and aesthetic preferences of generation Y and the baby boom generation: testing user web site experience through eye tracking. Int J Electron Commer 15:4121–4158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy BR (2006) Fundamental issues in social robotics. Int Rev Inf Ethics 6(12):2006

    Google Scholar 

  • Edmonds E, Muller L, Connell M (2006) Visual communication. 5(3):307–322, Effects of facial expression on responsibility attribution by groups and individuals. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 9:587–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger R, Jones JR, Stinglhamber F, Shanock L, Randall AT (2005) Flow experiences at work: for high need achievers alone? J Organ Behav 26:755–775

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot 6(3–4):169–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelhardt CR, Bartholow BD, Kerr GT, Bushman BJ (2011) This is your brain on violent digital games: neural desensitization to violence predicts increased aggression following violent digital game exposure. J Exp Soc Psychol 47(5):1033–1036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engeser S, Rheinberg F (2008) Flow, performance and moderators of challenge-skill balance. Motivation and Emotion 32:158–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fasola F, Matarić MJ (2013) A socially assistive robot exercise coach for the elderly. J Hum Rob Interact 2(2):3

    Google Scholar 

  • Feil-Seifer DJ, Matarić MJ (2011) Ethical principles for socially assistive robotics. IEEE Robot Automat Mag 18(1):24–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson CJ (2007) The good, the bad and the ugly: a meta-analytic review of positive and negative effects of violent digital games. Psychiatr Q 78(4):309–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernaeus Y, Håkansson M, Jacobsson M, Ljungblad S (2010, June) How do you play with a robotic toy animal? a long-term study of pleo. In: Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Interaction Design and Children. ACM, pp 39–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi M, Kis A, Faragó T, Janiak M, Muszyński R, Miklósi A (2016) Humans attribute emotions to a robot that shows simple behavioural patterns borrowed from dog behaviour. Comp Hum Behav 59(June):411–419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galloway A, Brucker-Cohen J, Gaye L, Goodman E, Hill D (2004) Design for hackability. In: Proceeding DIS’04. ACM Press, New York, NY, pp 363–366

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrison J (2003) Dewey’s Theory of emotions: the unity of thought and emotion in naturalistic functional “co-ordination” of behavior. Trans Charles S Peirce Soc 39(3):405–443

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin ET (2006) Befindlichkeit: Heidegger and the philosophy of psychology. Rev Existential Psychol Psychiatry 16(3):1–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Giambra LM (1979) Sex differences in daydreaming and related mental activity from the late teens to the early nineties. Int J Aging Hum Dev 10:1–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra LM, Grodsky A (1989) Task-unrelated images and thoughts while reading. Imagery Curr Perspect, 26–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Govers PCM, HekkertP, Schoormans JPL (2004) Happy, cute and tough: can designers create a product personality that consumers understand?. In MacDonagh D, Hekkert P, Van Erp J, Gyi D (eds) Design and emotion, the experience of everyday things, Taylor & Francis, London, pp 345–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Green C, Bavelier D (2003) Action digital game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423:534–537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruberger M, Simon EB, Levkovitz Y, Zangen A, Hendler T (2011) Towards a neuroscience of mind-wandering. Front Hum Neurosci 5:56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie SE (1997) Anthropomorphism: a definition and a theory. In: Mitchell RW, Thompson NS, Miles HL (eds) Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals. State University of New York Press, Albany, New York, pp 50–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Han S, Song Y, Ding Y, Yund EW, Woods DL (2001) Neural substrates for visual perceptual grouping in humans. Psychophysiology 38(6):926–935

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassenzahl M, Ullrich D (2007) To do or not to do: differences in user experience and retrospective judgments depending on the presence or absence of instrumental goals. Interact Comp 19(4):429–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger M (1992) History of the concept of time: Prolegomena (vol 717). Indiana university press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hekkert P, Wieringen PCW (1990) Complexity and prototypicality as determinants of the appraisal of cubist paintings. Br J Psychol 81(4):483–495

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu Ming-Chieh (2013) Annotation of dynamic identities in interactive aesthetics. Adv J Commun 01:441–449

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu S-H, Chou C-Y, Chen F-C, Wang Y-K, Chan T-W (2007) An investigation of the differences between robot and virtual learning companions’ influences on students’ engagement. The First IEEE International Workshop on Digital Game and Intelligent Toy Enhanced Learning, pp 41–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang L, Morency LP, Gratch J (2011) Virtual Rapport 2.0[C]//International Workshop on Intelligent Virtual Agents. Springer Berlin Heidelberg, pp 68–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 3(3):e79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson RD, Marakas GM, Palmer JW (2008) Beliefs about the social roles and capabilities of computing technology: development of the computing technology continuum of perspective. Behav Inf Technol 27(2):169–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn PH Jr, Ishiguro H, Friedman B, Kanda T, Freier NG, Severson RL, Miller J (2007) What is a human? toward psychological benchmarks in the field of human–robot interaction. Interact Stud 8(3):363–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kätsyri J, Förger K, Mäkäräinen M, Takala T (2015) A review of empirical evidence on different uncanny valley hypotheses: support for perceptual mismatch as one road to the valley of eeriness. Front Psychol 6:390. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller J, Bless H (2008) Flow and regulatory compatibility: an experimental approach to flow model of intrinsic motivation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34:196–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller J, Blomann F (2008) Locus of control and the flow experience. An experimental analysis. Eur J Pers 22:589–607

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler S, Hinds P (2004) Introduction to this special issue on human-robot interaction. Hum Comp Interact 19(1–2):1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330(6006):932

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim J, Lee J, Choi D (2003) Designing emotionally evocative homepages: an empirical study of the quantitative relations between design factors and emotional dimensions. Int J Hum Comp Stud 59(6):899–940

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kujala S, Miron-Shatz T (2015) The evolving role of expectations in long-term user experience. In: Proceedings of the 19th International Academic Mindtrek Conference. ACM, pp 167–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Lankes M, Riegler S, Weiss A, Mirlacher T, Pirker M, Tscheligi M (2008) Facial expressions as game input with different emotional feedback conditions. In: Proceeding ACE ’08, ACM, pp 253–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Leakey LSB, Tobias PV, Napier JR (1964) A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature 202:7–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee S, Sundar SS (2015) Cosmetic customization of mobile phones: cultural antecedents, psychological correlates. Media Psychol 18(1):1–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee YK, Chang CT, Lin Y, Cheng ZH (2014) The dark side of smartphone usage: psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress. Comp Hum Behav 31:373–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis MD (2005) Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behav Brain Sci 28(2):169–194

    Google Scholar 

  • Li DD, Liau AK, Khoo A (2013) Player-avatar identification in digital gaming: concept and measurement. Comp Hum Behav 29(1):257–263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu W, Pasman G, Taal-Fokker J, Stappers P (2013) Exploring ‘Generation Y’ interaction qualities at home and at work. Cogn Technol Work 16(3):405–415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locher PJ, Nodine CF (1987) Symmetry catches the eye. Eye movements: from physiology to cognition. In: O’Regan J, Levy-Schoen A (eds), Elsevier Science Publishers BV

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge M, McGraw KM, Stroh P (1989) An impression-driven model of candidate evaluation. Am Polit Sci Rev 83(2):399–419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus GR, Loftus EF (1983) Mind at play: the psychology of digital games. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Looser CE, Wheatley T (2010) The tipping point of animacy how, when, and where we perceive life in a face. Psychol Sci 21(12):1854–1862. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0956797610388044 // lots of face-based references

  • Low SM, Altman I (1992) Place attachment. In: Place attachment. Springer US, pp 1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludvigsen M, Petersen MG, Iversen O, Krogh P (2004) Aesthetic interaction: a pragmatists aesthetics of interactive systems

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz C, White GM (1986) The anthropology of emotions. Ann Rev Anthropol 15:405–436

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDorman KF, Ishiguro H (2006) The uncanny advantage of using androids in cognitive and social science research. Interact Stud 7:297–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDorman KF, Green RD, Ho C, Koch CT (2009) Too real for comfort? uncanny responses to computer generated faces. Comp Hum Behav 25:695–710. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean A, Carter K, Lövstrand L, Moran T (1990) User-tailorable systems: pressing the issues with buttons. Proceedings CHI ‘90. ACM Press, pp 175–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahlke M, Thüring M (2006) Measuring multiple components of emotions in interactive contexts. CHI’06 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems. ACM

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler JM (1978) A code in the node: the use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse Process 1(1):14–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Margolin V (1997) Getting to know the user. Des Stud 18(3):227–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marin MM, Leder H (2016) Effects of presentation duration on measures of complexity in affective environmental scenes and representational paintings. Acta Psychologica, 16338–16358

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayes DK, Cotton JE (2001) Measuring engagement in digital games: a questionnaire. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 45th Annual Meeting, pp 692–696

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin DL, Lynch EB, Coley JD, Atran S (1997) Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: do all roads lead to Rome? Cogn Psychol 32(1):49–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milgram S (1967) The small world problem. Psychol Today 2:60–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison AJ, Mitchell P, Brereton M (2007) The lens of ludic engagement: evaluating participation in interactive art installations. In: Proceedings of the 15th International Conference on Multimedia. ACM New York, NY, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek MD, Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2012) Mindfulness and mind-wandering: finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion 12:442–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukherjee S (2015) Digital games and storytelling: reading games and playing books. Palgrave MacMillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Nass C, Isbister K, Lee EJ (2000) Truth is beauty: researching embodied conversational agents. Embodied Conversational Agents, 374–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser U (2014) Cognitive psychology: Classic edition. Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman MW, Sedivy JZ, Neuwirth CM, Edwards WK, Hong JI, Izadi S, Marcelo K, Smith TF (2002) Designing for serendipity: supporting end-user configuration of ubiquitous computing environments. In: Proceedings of DIS ‘02, pp 147–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols S, Stich S (2005) Mindreading: a cognitive theory of pretense. Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen M, Dissanayake C (2000) An investigation of pretend play, mental state terms and false belief understanding: in search of a metarepresentational link. Br J Dev Psychol 18(4):609–624

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman DA (2002) Emotion & design: attractive things work better. Interactions 9(4):36–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman D, Miller J, Henderson A (1995) What you see, some of what’s in the future, and how we go about doing it: HI at Apple Computer. In: Conference companion, CHI 1995. ACM, p 155

    Google Scholar 

  • Overbeeke K, Wensveen S (2004) Beauty in use. Hum Comput Interact 19(4):367–369

    Google Scholar 

  • Overbeeke K, Wensveen S (2003) From perception to experience, from affordances to irresistibles. In Proceedings of the 2003 international conference on Designing pleasurable products and interfaces, pp 92–97

    Google Scholar 

  • Papert S (1980) Mindstorms: children, computers and powerful ideas. Basic Books, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelowski M, Markey PS, Lauring JO, Leder H (2016) Visualizing the impact of art: an update and comparison of current psychological models of art experience. Front Hum Neurosci 10

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle ME, Snyder AZ (2007) A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37(4):1083–1090

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratan R (2012) Self-presence, explicated: body, emotion, and identity extension into the virtual self. In: Handbook of Research on Technoself: Identity in a Technological Society. IGI Global, pp 321–335

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratan R, Hasler BS (2010) Exploring self-presence in collaborative virtual teams

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe M (2002) Heidegger’s attunement and the neuropsychology of emotion. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 1:287–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinecke K, Yeh T, Miratrix L, Mardiko R, Zhao Y, Liu J, Gajos KZ (2013, April) Predicting users’ first impressions of website aesthetics with a quantification of perceived visual complexity and colorfulness. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM, pp 2049–2058

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein R (2015) A short history of psychological perspective of emotion. In: Calvo RA, D’Mello S, Gratch J, Kappas A (eds) The Oxford handbook of affective computing. Oxford University Press, USA, pp 21–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezasyah E, Mariane N, Toyong P, Binti Mokhtar S (2012) Redefining needs. In: 2012 IEEE Symposium on Business, Engineering and Industrial Applications, pp 579–584

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozendaal MC, Keyson DV, de Ridder H (2007) Product behavior and appearance effects on experienced engagement during experiential and goal-directed tasks. In: Proceedings of the Conference on Designing Pleasurable Products and Interfaces, Helsinki, Finland, pp 181–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell JA, Pratt G (1980) A description of the affective quality attributed to environments. J Pers Soc Psychol 38(2):1259–1283

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabanovic S, Michalowski M, Simmons R (2006) Robots in the wild: observing human-robot social interaction outside the lab. In: Proceedings of AMC’06, Istanbul, pp 596–601

    Google Scholar 

  • Sable P (1995) Pets, attachment, and well-being across the life cycle. Soc Work 40(3):334–341

    Google Scholar 

  • Sageng JR, Fossheim HJ, Larsen TM (eds) (2012) The philosophy of computer games (vol 7). Springer Science & Business Media

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter S, Singer J (1962) Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev 69(5):379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer KR, Bänziger T, Roesch E (2010) A blueprint for affective computing: a sourcebook and manual. Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk N, Aziz DR, Bates MJB (2009) Attentional-associative interactions in creativity. Creat Res J 21(1):92–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sestir MA, Bartholow BD (2010) Violent and nonviolent digital games produce opposing effects on aggressive and prosocial outcomes. J Exp Soc Psychol 46(6):934–942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyama J, Nagayama RS (2007) The uncanny valley: effect of realism on the impression of artificial human faces. Pres Teleoperators Virtual Env 16:337–351. https://doi.org/10.1162/pres.16.4.337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sio UN, Ormerod TC (2009) Does incubation enhance problem solving? a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 135(1):94–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2006) The restless mind. Psychol Bull 132(6):946

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinosa C, Flores F, Dreyfus H (2001) Disclosing new worlds. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinfeld A, Fong T, Kaber D, Lewis M, Scholtz J, Schultz A, Goodrich M (2006, March) Common metrics for human-robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the 1st ACM SIGCHI/SIGART conference on Human-robot interaction. ACM, pp 33–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorsteinsson G, Page T (2014) User attachment to smartphones and design guidelines. Int J Mob Learn Organ 8(3–4):201–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tractinsky N, Shoval-Katz A, Ikar D (2000) What is beautiful is usable. Interact Comp 13:127–145

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle S (2008) Always-on/always-on-you: the tethered self. Handbook of mobile communication studies, p 121

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle S, Breazeal C, Dasté O, Scassellati B (2006) Encounters with kismet and cog: children respond to relational artifacts. Digital media: transformations in human communication, p 120

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner P (2016) HCI Redux: the promise of post-cognitive interaction. Springer

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner P, Wilson L, Turner S (2009) Do web pages have personalities? In: Proceedings European Conference on Cognitive Ergonomics. Helsinki

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang W (2017) Smartphones as social actors? social dispositional factors in assessing anthropomorphism. Comp Hum Behav 68:334–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson D, Clark AL, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 6:1063–1070

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werbach K, Hunter D (2012) For the win: how game thinking can revolutionize your business. Wharton Digital Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside J, Wixon D (1987, January) Improving human-computer interaction—a quest for cognitive science. In: Interfacing thought: Cognitive aspects of human-computer interaction. MIT Press, pp 353–365

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside J, Wixon D (1987) The dialectic of usability engineering. Proc INTERACT 87:17–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolley A, Kostopoulou O (2013) Clinical intuition in family medicine: more than first impressions. Ann Fam Med 11(1):60–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xenakis I, Arnellos A, Darzentas J (2012) The functional role of emotions in aesthetic judgement. New Ideas Psychol 30(2):212–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Złotowski JA, Sumioka H, Nishio S, Glas DF, Bartneck C, Ishiguro H (2015) Persistence of the uncanny valley: the influence of repeated interactions and a robot’s attitude on its perception. Front Psychol 6(883):1–13

    Google Scholar 

Web References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Phil Turner .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Turner, P. (2017). Killing Time with Technology. In: A Psychology of User Experience . Human–Computer Interaction Series. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-70653-5_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-70653-5_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-70652-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-70653-5

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics