Spatially Oriented Behaviour
Chapter
First Online:
- 325 Downloads
Abstract
In order to interact with the world around us, we need to know where objects are located (spatial perception), how to get there (spatial navigation) and how to appropriately exert force on the objects (spatial object manipulation). This chapter shows that spatial perception and spatial object manipulation may be affected differently by weightlessness, highlights the challenges of spatial navigation with unrestricted degrees of freedom and discusses several alternatives for preflight training of spatially oriented behaviour.
Keywords
Spatial orientation Wayfinding Motor performance Sensorimotor coordinationReferences
- Aoki H, Ohno R, Yamaguchi T (2005) The effect of the configuration and the interior design of a virtual weightless space station on human spatial orientation. Acta Astronaut 56:1005–1016CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Aoki H, Oman CM, Natapoff A (2007) Virtual-reality-based 3D navigation training for emergency egress from spacecraft. Aviat Space Environ Med 78:774–783PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bloomberg J, Bock O (2012) Adaptation to weightlessness. In: Seel N (ed) Encyclopedia of the sciences of learning. Springer, Berlin, pp 102–103Google Scholar
- Bock O (1998) Problems of sensorimotor coordination in weightlessness. Brain Res Rev 28:155–160CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bock O (2013) Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings. Front Hum Neurosci 7:81CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bock O, Bury N (2016) Flipping a switch “down” when not aligned with the gravitational vertical. Aerosp Med Hum Perform 87:838–843CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bock O, Weigelt C, Bloomberg JJ (2010) Cognitive demand of human sensorimotor performance during an extended space mission. A dual-task study. Aviat Space Environ Med 81:819–824CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bock O, Schott N, Papaxanthis C (2015) Motor imagery: lessons learned in movement science might be applicable for spaceflight. Front Syst Neurosci 9:75CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bolender H, Stevenin H, Bessone L, Torres A (2006) Preparing for space: EVA training at the European Astronaut Centre. ESA Bull 128:32–40Google Scholar
- Bullens J, Iglói K, Berthoz A, Postma A, Rondi-Reig L (2010) Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies. J Exp Child Psychol 107:337–350CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Burns PC (1999) Navigation and the mobility of older drivers. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 54:S49–S55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Burr DC, Morrone MC, Ross J (2001) Separate visual representations for perception and action revealed by saccadic eye movements. Curr Biol 11:798–802CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bury N, Bock O (2016) Role of gravitational versus egocentric cues for human spatial orientation. Exp Brain Res 234:1013–1018CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Campbell MR, Williams DR, Buckey JR, Kirkpatrick AW (2005) Animal surgery during spaceflight on the Neurolab shuttle mission. Aviat Space Environ Med 76:589–593PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clément G, Ngo-Anh JT (2013) Space physiology II: adaptation of the central nervous system to space flight—past, current, and future studies. Eur J Appl Physiol 113:1655–1672CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Glasauer S, Mittelstaedt H (1992) Determinants of orientation in microgravity. IAA Man Space Symp 27:1–9Google Scholar
- Glasauer S, Mittelstaedt H (1998) Perception of spatial orientation in microgravity. Brain Res Rev 28:185–193CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Goodale MA, Meenan JP, Bulthoff HH, Nicolle DA, Murphy KJ, Racicot CI (1994) Separate neural pathway for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension. Curr Biol 4:604–610CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Gresty MA, Golding JF, Le H, Nightingale K (2008) Cognitive impairment by spatial disorientation. Aviat Space Environ Med 79:105–111CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Haffenden AM, Schiff KC, Goodale MA (2001) The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp. Curr Biol 11:177–181CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Harm DL, Parker DE (1993) Perceived self-orientation and self-motion in microgravity, after landing and during preflight adaption training. J Vestib Res 3:297–305PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Holmes PS, Collins DJ (2001) The PETTLEP approach to motor imagery: a functional equivalence model for sport psychologists. J Appl Sport Psychol 13:60–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jenkin HL, Dyde RT, Zacher JE, Zikovitz DC, Jenkin ARS, Howard IP, Harris LR (2005) The relative role of visual and non-visual cues in determining the perceived direction of “up”: experiments in parabolic flight. Acta Astronaut 56:1025–1032CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kalicinski M, Kempe M, Bock O (2015) Motor imagery: effects of age, task complexity, and task setting. Exp Aging Res 41(1):25–38CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kalicinski M, Steinberg F, Dalecki M, Bock O (2016) Gaze behavior while operating a complex instrument-control task. Aerosp Med Hum Perform 87:1–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kornilova LN (1997) Orientation illusions in spaceflight. J Vestib Res 7:429–439CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kubis JF, McLaughlin EJ, Jackson JM, Rusnak D, McBride GH, Saxon SV (1977) Task and work performance on Skylab missions 2, 3, and 4. Time and motion study – experiment M151. In: Biomedical results from Skylab. National Aeronautics and Space Administration, Washington, pp 136–154Google Scholar
- Lackner JR, DiZio P (1993) Multisensory, cognitive, and motor influence on human spatial orientation in weightlessness. J Vestib Res 3:361–372PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lackner JR, DiZio P (2000) Human orientation and movement control in weightless and artificial gravity environments. Exp Brain Res 130:2–26CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lackner JR, Graybiel A (1979) Parabolic flight: loss of sense of orientation. Science 206:1105–1108CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Loftin RB, Kenney PJ, Benedetti R, Culbert C, Engelberg M, Jones R, Saito T (1994) Virtual environments in training: NASA’s hubble space telescope mission. In: Interservice/industry training systems & education conferenceGoogle Scholar
- Manzey D, Lorenz B, Schiewe A, Finell G, Thiele G (1995) Dual-task performance in space. Results from a single-case study during a short-term space mission. Hum Factors 37:667–681CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Matsnev EI, Yakovleva IY, Tarasov IK, Alekseev VN, Kornilova LN, Mateev AD, Gorgiladze GI (1983) Space motion sickness: phenomenology, countermeasures, and mechanisms. Aviat Space Environ Med 54:312–317PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Meneghetti C, Borella E, Fiore F, de Beni R (2013) The ability to point to well-known places in young and older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res 25:203–209CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG, Macko KA (1983) Object vision and spatial vision. Two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci 6:414–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Moffat SD, Resnick SM (2002) Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. Behav Neurosci 116:851CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- NASA (1999) International space station flight crew integration standard (NASA–STD–3000/T). SSP 50005Google Scholar
- Oman CM (2003) Human visual orientation in weightlessness. In: Harris LR, Jenkin M (eds) Levels of perception. Springer, Berlin, pp 375–398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Oman C (2007) Spatial orientation and navigation in microgravity. In: Mast F, Jäncke L (eds) Spatial processing in navigation, imagery and perception. Springer, Berlin, pp 369–387Google Scholar
- Oman CM, Lichtenberg BK, Money KE, McCoy RK (1986) M.I.T./Canadian vestibular experiments on the Spacelabe-1 mission. 4. Space motion sickness: symptoms, stimuli, and predictability. Exp Brain Res 64:316–334CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Oman CM, Shebilske WL, Richards JT, Tubré TC, Beall AC, Natapoff A (2000) Three dimensional spatial memory and learning in real and virtual environments. Spat Cogn Comput 2:355–372Google Scholar
- Paillard J (1991) Motor and representational framing of space. In: Brain Space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 163–182Google Scholar
- Rafiq A, Broderick TJ, Williams DCR, Jones JA, Merrell RC (2005) Assessment of simulated surgical skills in parabolic microgravity. Aviat Space Environ Med 76:385–391PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rock I (1954) The perception of the egocentric orientation of a line. J Exp Psychol 48:367–374CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rodgers MK, Sindone JA, Moffat SD (2012) Effects of age on navigation strategy. Neurobiol Aging 33:e15–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Siegel AW, White SH (1975) The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. Adv Child Dev Behav 10:9–55CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sigman E, Goodenough DR, Flannangan M (1979) Instructions, illusory self-tilt and the rod-and-frame test. Q J Exp Psychol 31:155–165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Steinberg F, Kalicinski M, Dalecki M, Bock O (2015) Human performance in a realistic instrument-control task during short-term microgravity. PLoS One 10:e0128992CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Stroud KJ, Harm DL, Klaus DM (2005) Preflight virtual reality training as a countermeasure for space motion sickness and disorientation. Aviat Space Environ Med 76:352–356PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Tafforin C, Campan R (1994) Ethological experiments on human orientation behavior within a three-dimensional space-in microgravity. Adv Space Res 14:415–418CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. American Psychological Association, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
- Vidal M, Amorim M, Berthoz A (2004) Navigating in a virtual three-dimensional maze: how do egocentric and allocentric reference frames interact? Cogn Brain Res 19:244–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Witkin HA (1949) Perception of body position and of the position of the visual field. Psychol Monogr Gen Appl 63:1–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Young LR, Oman CM, Watt DG, Money KE, Lichtenberg BK, Kenyon RV, Arrott AP (1986) M.I.T./Canadian vestibular experiments on the Spacelab-1 mission: 1. Sensory adaptation to weightlessness and readaptation to one-g: an overview. Exp Brain Res 64:291–298PubMedGoogle Scholar
Copyright information
© The Author(s) 2017