Skip to main content

What Drives Citizen Perceptions of Government Corruption? National Income, Petty Bribe Payments and the Unknown

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: International Economic Association Series ((IEA))

Abstract

Low trust in government and the widespread sense that public institutions are corrupt appears to be a global phenomenon, and a challenge to effective governance. We use the Global Corruption Barometer with responses from individuals in 117 countries to probe what drives citizen perceptions of corruption and propensity to pay bribes. Our analysis of the role of particular individual characteristics on perceptions of corruption (including age, gender, self-reported income level, education and employment status) suggests that there is at least somewhat of a common sense across individuals of how corrupt a particular service is in their country, but other—largely unobservable—factors account for most of any particular individual’s responses. A second analysis of the relationship between country averages of perceptions of corruption and country-level explanatory variables (including GDP per capita, democracy, inequality, the probability of bribe payment as well as a service performance measure for each service) shows a strong negative association between GDP per capita and perceptions of corruption and a strong positive association between bribe payments and perceptions of corruption. We find little evidence of association between service performance measures and country averages of corruption perceptions in a given sector. We repeat the same analyses on the individual and the country levels using bribe payment as our dependent variable. Our findings suggest that GDP per capita lowers perceptions of corruption through its influence on bribe payments. A plausible conclusion from that interpretation would be that the one reliable tool we appear to have to reduce perceptions of corruption is development and associated increases in GDP per capita itself.

We are grateful for assistance from Ben Crisman.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Notes

  1. 1.

    See for example: Diamond (2007), Gallup (2015) or Whiteley et al. (2015).

  2. 2.

    Solé-Ollé and Sorribas-Navarro (2014).

  3. 3.

    Anderson (2015); Daude et al. (2012).

  4. 4.

    Mungiu-Pippidi (2015) distinguishes between particularistic and personalistic societies where corruption is the norm, and those where norm-based “ethical universalism” is the default.

  5. 5.

    Individual perceptions of corruption have been found to be consistently affected by factors including age, ethnicity, education, political affiliation (Olken 2009).

  6. 6.

    Note that reported bribe-paying covers both a bribe payment by the survey respondent herself/himself or anyone living in their household. The question posed to respondents with regards to bribes is: “In the past 12 months, have you or anyone living in your household paid a bribe in any form to each of the following institutions/organizations?”

  7. 7.

    Mostly 2010 for perceptions of corruption about political parties, education, the judiciary and the police and mostly 2007 for perceptions of corruption about medical services, permits, utilities and tax services.

  8. 8.

    Mostly 2010 for most countries the country-level overall corruption perception variable is the average of perceptions about the education services, the judiciary and the police—weighted by the number of respondents. For some countries—depending on the data available in the most recent survey year—the overall corruption perception variable also includes medical-, permit-, utilities- and tax services. Perceptions about political parties are not included for any country.

  9. 9.

    See: Cole and Gramajo (2009), Fajnzylber et al. (2002), Neumayer (2003).

  10. 10.

    For a detailed description of these measures, see: https://www.freedomhouse.org/report/freedom-world-2015/methodology#.UuEq87Qo71I (Freedom House) and http://www.systemicpeace.org/polityproject.html (Polity 2 scores).

  11. 11.

    Given that experts are specifically asked to consider bribes and corruption in their assessment of Freedom House’s individual rights measure, any causal argument regarding this measure and our corruption indicators would have to be made with considerable care.

  12. 12.

    Between two consecutive survey years or between 2007 and 2009 (we have no 2008 observations in our data).

  13. 13.

    Azfar and Murrell (2009); Kraay and Murrell (2013).

  14. 14.

    As categorized by the World Bank in FY 2016.

  15. 15.

    Only regression tables with results for all countries (HICs and non-HICs combined) are included in the Appendix. Separate regression results tables for HICs and non-HICs are available from the authors on request.

  16. 16.

    The average across all services is 2.9 for HICs and 3.5 for non-HICs. The differences for all sectors are statistically significant using a two-sample t test with unequal variance.

  17. 17.

    For example, as countries get richer and more educated, citizens become more sensitive to corruption possibilities; as countries get even more rich and educated, perception of corruption decline, presumably as government services improve overall. We find a hint of this possibility in the regression results below, where the education effect is positive on perceptions in developing countries and negative in the high-income countries.

  18. 18.

    In a bivariate regression with perceptions of corruption across sectors as the dependent variable and log GDP per capita as the right-hand side variable.

  19. 19.

    In the case of political parties, the average perception of corruption about political parties is a noteworthy exception to this trend: GDP per capita is not statistically significantly associated with perceptions but—unlike in other services—the average ten-year GDP growth is. Perhaps growth is a good “outcome variable” for political parties. Using three-year growth, the result was not significant for low- and middle-income countries—results available from authors.

  20. 20.

    Using top 10% income share as a measure of inequality produces similar results—available from the authors on request.

  21. 21.

    We also examine the relationship between our country-level perceptions measure and other measures of corruption—the CPIA, expert responses to Bertelsmann Transformation Index’s (BTI) questions on impunity and corruption control, and expert responses to three questions regarding corruption in awarding jobs, business licenses and procurement contracts from the Quality of Government Institute’s (QoG) 2012 Expert Survey. We see no statistically significant correlation between the CPIA or the corruption-related BTI questions, but the three QoG assessments of corruption do show a significant association with our (GCB) country-level citizen perception aggregates. One potential explanation could be that the QoG questions we tested focus on specific instances of corruption (getting a job, getting a business license, procurement contract), which may be more aligned with citizens’ own experiences than indicators that assess government corruption overall (CPIA, BTI). Results available on request.

  22. 22.

    At the same time, some bribe payments may not represent corruption but a misunderstanding on the part of citizens, for example if citizens report a payment to a head teacher that is voluntary for extra school supplies as mandatory and thus a bribe. This is what not all of the bribe-paying is included in the “actual corruption” circle.

  23. 23.

    Dal Bo and Di Tella (2003).

  24. 24.

    The authors could construct a simple dummy variable to capture whether the previous year there was a significant corruption scandal and whether this led to any action by the government.

  25. 25.

    The difference in implementation of laws within a country will also lead to a difference in perceptions of corruption by citizens.

  26. 26.

    This is in line with the “resistance to reforms” argument first highlighted by Fernandez and Rodrik (1991).

References

  • Anderson, John E. 2015 Trust in Government and the Willingness to Pay Taxes In Transition Countries. Paper Prepared for Presentation at the 71st Annual Congress of the International Institute of Public Finance (IIPF), August 20–23, 2015, Dublin, Ireland. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2652268

  • Azfar, Omar, and Peter Murrell. 2009. Identifying Reticent Respondents: Assessing the Quality of Survey Data On Corruption and Values. Economic Development and Cultural Change 57: 387–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, Julio H., and Andres Marroquin Gramajo. 2009. Homicide Rates in a Cross-Section of Countries: Evidence and Interpretations. Population and Development Review 35: 749–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bo, Dal, Ernesto, and Rafael Di Tella. 2003. Capture by Threat. Journal of Political Economy 111: 1123–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daude, Christian, Gutiérrez, Hamlet and Ángel Melguizo. 2012. What Drives Tax Morale? Working Papers, No. 315, OECD Development Centre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, Larry. 2007. Building Trust in Government by Improving Governance. Paper Presented to the 7th Global Forum on Reinventing Government: “Building Trust in Government”. http://stanford.edu/~ldiamond/paperssd/BuildingTrustinGovernmentUNGLobalForum.pdf

  • Fajnzylber, Pablo, Daniel Lederman, and Norman Loayza. 2002. Inequality and Violent Crime. Journal of Law and Economics 45: 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez, Rachel, and Dani Rodrik. 1991. Resistance to Reform: Status Quo Bias in Presence of Individual Specific Uncertainty. American Economic Review 81: 1146–1155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup. 2015. Trust in Government. Gallup website. http://www.gallup.com/poll/5392/trust-government.aspx

  • Kraay, Aart and Peter Murrell. 2013. Misunderstanding Corruption. Research Working Paper 6488, World Bank Policy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mungiu-Pippidi, Alina. 2015. The Quest for Good Governance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Neumayer, Eric. 2003. Good Policy Can Lower Violent Crime: Evidence from a Cross-National Panel of Homicide Rates, 1980–97. Journal of Peace Research 40: 619-640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olken, Benjamin A. 2009. Corruption Perceptions vs. Corruption Reality. Journal of Public Economics 93: 950–964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose-Ackerman, Susan. 1999. Corruption and Government: Causes, Consequences and Reform. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Solé-Ollé, Albert, and Pilar Sorribas-Navarro. 2014. Do Corruption Scandals Erode Trust in Government? Evidence from a Matched Sample of Local Governments. Unpublished Paper Prepared for the CESifo Area Conference on Public Sector Economics, Munich. Vol. 2426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiteley, Paul, Harold D. Clarke, David Sanders, and Marianne Stewart. 2015. Why do Voters Lose Trust in Governments? Public Perceptions of Government Honesty and Trustworthiness in Britain 2000–2013. The British Journal of Politics and International Relations. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-856X.12073.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Kenny .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix

Appendix

Table 10.8 Variables—data and sources
Table 10.9 Country-level summary statistics (all countries)
Table 10.10 Country-level summary statistics: Average corruption perceptions by service
Table 10.11 Country-level summary statistics: probability of contact by service
Table 10.12 Country-level summary statistics: probability of bribe payment by service (conditional on contact)
Table 10.13 Individual-level summary statistics: respondent characteristics
Table 10.14 Individual-level summary statistics: contact dummies by service
Table 10.15 Individual-level summary statistics: bribe payment dummies by service
Table 10.16 Individual-level summary statistics: bribe payment as a dependent variablea by service

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Birdsall, N., Kenny, C., Diofasi, A. (2018). What Drives Citizen Perceptions of Government Corruption? National Income, Petty Bribe Payments and the Unknown. In: Basu, K., Cordella, T. (eds) Institutions, Governance and the Control of Corruption. International Economic Association Series. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65684-7_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65684-7_10

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-73822-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-65684-7

  • eBook Packages: Economics and FinanceEconomics and Finance (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics