Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Zooarchaeology

  • Diane Gifford-Gonzalez
Chapter

Abstract

Chapter 24 offers an overview of the history and basic premises of human behavioral ecology (HBE ), which as been especially widely used by zooarchaeologists studying foragers in the United States. It begins with a history of HBE as it emerged from zoology and through simple examples outlines classic rate maximization models of prey choice and foraging patch choice. Given that many zooarchaeological studies have used them, central place models (CPM ) are detailed, as are the complexities of resource depression, again given the many zooarchaeological studies investigating this phenomenon with archaeofaunal data. Used in zoology for several decades, dynamic state variable models (DSVM) of foraging diverge from “static” rate-maximization models in incorporating the forager’s state before foraging, using multiple currencies (e.g. calories and fat), and including more fitness-shaping factors such as risk of death in a patch in the model, while using iterative simulations to stochastically vary patch conditions in which the forager makes decisions. Actualistic and archaeological studies using DSVM and other dynamic models suggest that income variance reduction, rather than rate maximization, is the predicted “solution” for optimal fitness. The role of proxies and their sensitivities to factors other than those of the model are explored, with the specific example of the Artiodactyl Index. The most interesting aspect of HBE’s application in zooarchaeology lies its ability to systematically expose cases in which people do not behave like “optimal foragers” but instead opt – or perhaps are forced – to forage in some other way, as examples show. The difference between using HBE predictive models as tools for inquiry with a dataset versus as explanations of the data is discussed.

Keywords

Behavioral ecology HBE Currencies Return rate Handling Encounter rate Patch choice Rate maximization Reliability DSVM 

References

  1. Anderies, J. M. (1996). An adaptive model for predicting !Kung reproductive performance: A stochastic dynamic programming approach. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(4), 221–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bayham, F. E. (1982). A diachronic analysis of prehistoric animal exploitation at Ventana cave. Dissertation: Arizona State University, Tempe, Ann Arbor, MI, University Microfilms.Google Scholar
  3. Belovsky, G. E. (1978). Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: The moose. Theoretical Population Biology, 14(1), 105–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Belovsky, G. E., & Jordan, P. A. (1981). Sodium dynamics and adaptations of a moose population. Journal of Mammalogy, 62(3), 613–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bettinger, R. L. (2009). Hunter-gatherer foraging: Five simple models. Clinton Corners: Elliott Werner Publications.Google Scholar
  6. Bettinger, R. L., Malhi, R., & McCarthy, H. (1997). Central place models of acorn and mussel processing. Journal of Archaeological Science, 24(10), 887–899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bettinger, R. L., Winterhalder, B., & McElreath, R. (2006). A simple model of technological intensification. Journal of Archaeological Science, 33(4), 538–545.Google Scholar
  8. Bettinger, R. L., Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2009). Constraints on the development of agriculture. Current Anthropology, 50(5), 627–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. L. (1997). Contemporary shellfish gathering strategies among the Meriam of the Torres Strait islands, Australia: Testing predictions of a central place foraging model. Journal of Archaeological Science, 24(1), 39–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. (2000). The ethnoarchaeology of juvenile foragers: Shellfishing strategies among Meriam children. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 19(4), 461–476.Google Scholar
  11. Bird, D. W., & O’Connell, J. F. (2006). Behavioral ecology and archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Research, 14(2), 143–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bird, D. W., & O'Connell, J. F. (2012). Human behavioral ecology. In I. Hodder (Ed.), Archaeological theory today (2nd ed., pp. 37–61). Malden: Polity Press.Google Scholar
  13. Bird, D. W., Codding, B. F., Bliege Bird, R., Zeanah, D. W., & Taylor, C. J. (2013). Megafauna in a continent of small game: Archaeological implications of Martu camel hunting in Australia's Western Desert. Quaternary International, 297, 155–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Boone, C. M. (2012). Integrating zooarchaeology and modeling: Trans-Holocene fishing in Monterey Bay, California. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
  15. Broughton, J. M. (1997). Widening diet breadth, declining foraging efficiency, and prehistoric harvest pressure: Ichthyofaunal evidence from the Emeryville Shellmound, California. Antiquity, 71(274), 845–862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Broughton, J. M. (2002). Prey spatial structure and behavior affect archaeological tests of optimal foraging models: Examples from the Emeryville Shellmound vertebrate fauna. World Archaeology, 34(1), 60–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Broughton, J. M., & O’Connell, J. F. (1999). On evolutionary ecology, selectionist archaeology, and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity, 64(1), 153–165.Google Scholar
  18. Burkhardt, R. W. (2005). Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  19. Butler, V. L. (2000). Resource depression on the Northwest Coast of North America. Antiquity, 74(285), 649–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Butler, V. L. (2001). Changing fish use on Mangaia, southern Cook Islands: Resource depression and the prey choice model. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology, 11(1–2), 88–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Charnov, E. L. (1976a). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9(2), 129–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Charnov, E. L. (1976b). Optimal foraging: Attack strategy of a mantid. American Naturalist, 110(971), 141–151.Google Scholar
  23. Charnov, E. L., Orians, G. H., & Hyatt, K. (1976). Ecological implications of resource depression. American Naturalist, 110(972), 247–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Clark, C. W., & Mangel, M. (1984). Foraging and flocking strategies: Information in an uncertain environment. American Naturalist, 123(5), 626–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Clark, C. W., & Mangel, M. (2000). Dynamic state variable models in ecology: Methods and applications. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Codding, B. F., Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. L. (2010). Interpreting abundance indices: Some aooarchaeological implications of Martu foraging. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37(12), 3200–3210.Google Scholar
  27. Dignan, C., Burlingame, B., Kumar, S., & Aalbersberg, W. (2004). The Pacific Islands food composition tables (2nd ed.). Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.Google Scholar
  28. Gifford-Gonzalez, D., Gobalet, K., Gaeta, J., & Geary, J. C. (2006). The faunal sample from CA-SMA-18: Environment, subsistence, taphonomy, historical ecology. In M. Hylkema, W. Hildebrandt, J. Farquhar, D. Gifford-Gonzalez, & K. Gobalet (Eds.), Archaeological investigations at CA-SMA-18: A study of prehistoric adaptations at Año Nuevo State Reserve. Report prepared for California Department of Parks and Recreation, Santa Cruz District (pp. 29–50). Davis: Far Western Anthropological Research Group.Google Scholar
  29. Gremillion, K. J., Barton, L., & Piperno, D. R. (2014a). Particularism and the retreat from theory in the archaeology of agricultural origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(17), 6171–6177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gremillion, K. J., Barton, L., & Piperno, D. R. (2014b). Reply to Smith: On distinguishing between models, hypotheses, and theoretical frameworks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(28), E2830.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408909111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hildebrandt, W., Rosenthal, J., & Gmoser, G. (2009). Shellfish transport, caloric return rates, and prehistoric feasting on the Laguna de Santa Rosa, Alta California. California Archaeology, 1(1), 55–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hockett, B. S. (2016). Why celebrate the death of primitive economic man?: Human nutritional ecology in the 21st century. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 5, 617–621.Google Scholar
  33. Hockett, B. S., & Haws, J. A. (2003). Nutritional ecology and diachronic trends in Palaeolithic diet and health. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 211–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Houston, A., & McNamara, J. (1985). The choice of two prey types that minimises the probability of starvation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17(2), 135–141.Google Scholar
  35. Houston, A., Clark, C., McNamara, J., & Mangel, M. (1988). Dynamic models in behavioural and evolutionary ecology. Nature, 332(6159), 29–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jones, J. H., Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2013). To kill a kangaroo: Understanding the decision to pursue high-risk/high-gain resources. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1767), 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1992). The evolutionary ecology of food acquisition. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 167–202). Hawthorne: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  38. Kennett, D. J., & Winterhalder, B. (Eds.). (2006). Human behavioral ecology and the origins of food production. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  39. Klein, R. G., & Cruz-Uribe, K. (1983). Stone Age population numbers and average tortoise size at Byneskranskop Cave 1 and Die Kelders Cave 1, Southern Cape Province, South Africa. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 38(137), 26–30.Google Scholar
  40. Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (Eds.). (2009). Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (4th ed.). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  41. Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, J. T., Webber, M. I., & Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major). Animal Behaviour, 25, 30–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Krebs, J. R., Davies, N. B., & West, S. A. (2012). An introduction to behavioural Ecology (4th ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
  43. Lupo, K. D. (2007). Evolutionary foraging models in zooarchaeological analysis: Recent applications and future challenges. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15(2), 143–189.Google Scholar
  44. Lupo, K. D., & Schmitt, D. N. (2005). Small prey hunting technology and zooarchaeological measures of taxonomic diversity and abundance: Ethnoarchaeological evidence from Central African forest foragers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 24(4), 335–353.Google Scholar
  45. Luttbeg, B., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Mangel, M. (2000). To marry again or not: A dynamic model for demographic transition. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 345–368). Hawthorne: Aldine De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  46. Lyman, R. L. (1989). Seal and sea lion hunting: A zooarchaeological study from the southern Northwest Coast of North America. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 8(1), 68–99.Google Scholar
  47. Lyman, R. L. (2003). Pinniped behavior, foraging theory, and the depression of metapopulations and nondepression of a local population in the southern Northwest Coast of North America. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 22(4), 376–388.Google Scholar
  48. Mace, R. (1989). Pastoralist strategies for survival in unpredictable environments: A model of herd composition that maximises household viability. Agricultural Systems, 31(2), 185–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mace, R. (1993). Nomadic pastoralists adopt subsistence strategies that maximise long-term household survival. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33(5), 329–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mace, R. (1996). When to have another baby: A dynamic model of reproductive decision-making and evidence from Gabbra pastoralists. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(4), 263–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Metcalfe, D., & Barlow, K. R. (1992). A model for exploring the optimal trade-off between field processing and transport. American Anthropologist, 94(2), 340–356.Google Scholar
  52. Moss, M. L. (2016). The nutritional value of Pacific herring: An ancient cultural keystone species on the Northwest Coast of North America. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 5, 649–655.Google Scholar
  53. Nagaoka, L. (2001). Using diversity indices to measure changes in prey choice at the Shag River Mouth Site, southern New Zealand. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology, 11(1–2), 101–111.Google Scholar
  54. Parker, G. A. (2006). Behavioural ecology: Natural history as science. In J. R. Lucas & L. W. Simmons (Eds.), Essays in animal behaviour: Celebrating 50 years of animal behaviour (pp. 23–56). Burlington: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
  55. Pulliam, H. R. (1981). On predicting human diets. Journal of Ethnobiology, 1(1), 61–68.Google Scholar
  56. Schmitt, D. N., & Lupo, K. D. (1995). On mammalian taphonomy, taxonomic diversity, and measuring subsistence data in zooarchaeology. American Antiquity, 60(3), 496–514.Google Scholar
  57. Schmitt, D. N., & Lupo, K. D. (2008). Do faunal remains reflect socioeconomic status? An ethnoarchaeological study among Central African farmers in the northern Congo Basin. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 27(3), 315–325.Google Scholar
  58. Smith, B. D. (2014). Failure of optimal foraging theory to appeal to researchers working on the origins of agriculture worldwide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(28), E2829.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408208111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Speth, J. D., & Scott, S. L. (1989). Horticulture and large mammal hunting: The role of resource depletion and the constraints of time and labor. In S. Kent (Ed.), Farmers as hunters: The implications of sedentism (pp. 71–79). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Stiner, M. C., Munro, N. D., & Surovell, T. A. (2000). The tortoise and the hare: Small-game use, the Broad-Spectrum Revolution, and Paleolithic demography. Current Anthropology, 41(1), 39–79.Google Scholar
  61. The R Project for Statistical Computing. (2017). https://www.r-project.org/. Accessed 2017.
  62. Thomas, D. H. (2014). The shellfishers of St. Catherines Island: Hardscrabble foragers or farming beachcombers? The Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 9(2), 169–182.Google Scholar
  63. Tullock, G. (1971). The coal tit as a careful shopper. American Naturalist, 105(941), 77–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Ugan, A. (2005a). Climate, bone density, and resource depression: What is driving variation in large and small game in Fremont archaeofaunas? Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 24(3), 227–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Ugan, A. (2005b). Does size matter? Body size, mass collecting, and their implications for understanding prehistoric foraging behavior. American Antiquity, 70(1), 75–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Ugan, A., Bright, J., & Rogers, A. (2003). When is technology worth the trouble? Journal of Archaeological Science, 30(10), 1315–1329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Whitaker, A. R. (2009). Are deer really susceptible to resource depression? California Archaeology, 1(1), 93–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Whitaker, A. R. (2010). Prehistoric behavioral depression of cormorant (Phalacrocorax spp.) on the northern California coast. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37(10), 2562–2571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Whitaker, A. R., & Byrd, B. F. (2014). Social circumscription, territoriality, and the late Holocene intensification of small-bodied shellfish along the California coast. Journal of Coastal and Island Archaeology, 9(2), 150–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Winterhalder, B., & Kennett, D. J. (2009). Four neglected concepts with a role to play in explaining the origins of agriculture. Current Anthropology, 50(5), 645–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (1981). Hunter-gatherer foraging strategies: Ethnographic and archaeological analyses. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  73. Zeder, M. A. (2014). Alternative to faith-based science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(28), E2827.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408209111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Diane Gifford-Gonzalez
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of CaliforniaSanta CruzUSA

Personalised recommendations