Skip to main content

Maternal Voice and Communicative Musicality: Sharing the Meaning of Life from Before Birth

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development

Abstract

A mother talking and playing with her infant a few weeks old shares delicate patterns of movement with her child, matching rhythms and expressions of emotion. The infant is an active, sensitive, and creative partner, often taking the lead. The dynamics and affections or emotions of their relating depend on innate adaptations for moving the human body rhythmically to sustain vitality and for communicating their regulations of an intelligent life by shared motives and feelings. Mothers’ affectionate speech is “musical” in ways adapted to fit with and celebrate the expressive impulses and feelings of the baby seeking company. Applying methods of acoustics physics, a musician Stephen Malloch developed a microanalysis of the “communicative musicality” both mother and infant enjoy in their vocal play. He traced how parameters of sound he identified in their voices were represented in proto-conversations of the first 2 months after a full-term birth and then how the impulse to share musical expressions invents playful rituals combining poetic narratives with actions of the body in universal human ways, which lead to learning of culture-specific habits and a language. Innate motives for the discovery of meaning in companionship that appear in the first efforts of a human self to share life in movement with affectionate friends constitute an essential support for any sensitive therapeutic care or education that adults may offer for a developing child, especially for a prematurely born baby (Trevarthen and Fresquez, Body, Movement and Dance in Psychotherapy, 2015; Trevarthen and Malloch, The Nordic Journal of Music Therapy 9(2), 3–17, 2000).

“Let the child lead the way”

Maori proverb, New Zealand

Trevarthen, C. (2017, in press) Maternal voice and communicative musicality: Sharing the meaning of life from before birth. For “Maternal Voice Intervention and Preterm Infant's Brain Development”. Edited by Manuela Filippa, Pierre Kuhn and Björn Westrup. Springer, Chapter 1.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bateson, M. C. (1979). The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 63–77). London, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boston Process Change Study Group. (2010). Change in psychotherapy: A unifying paradigm. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B. (1961). Psychophysiologic reactions in the neonate. I: The value of observations of the neonate. Journal of Pediatrics, 58, 508–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B. (1979). Evidence of communication during neonatal behavioural assessment. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 79–88). London, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York, NY/London, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1968). Processes of cognitive growth: Infancy. (Heinz Werner Lectures, 1968) Worcester, MA: Clark University Press with Barri Publishers. (Republished in G. Marsico (Ed.). (2015). Jerome S. Bruner beyond 100, cultural psychology of education 2. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-25536-1_2

  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk. Learning to use language. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castiello, U., Becchio, C., Zoia, S., Nelini, C., Sartori, L., Blason, L., … Gallese, V. (2010). Wired to be social: The ontogeny of human interaction. PLoS One, 5(10), e13199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. S. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183, 99–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delafield-Butt, J. T., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). The ontogenesis of narrative: From movements to meaning. Frontiers of Psychology, 6, 1157. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York, NY/London, England: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. Glasgow, Scotland: Fontana/Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M. (1992). Human minds: An exploration. London, England: Allen Lane/Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60, 1497–1510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodrich, B. G. (2010). We do, therefore we think: Time, motility, and consciousness. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 21, 331–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M. (1999). Expressions of belonging: The effect of acculturation on the rhythm and harmony of mother-infant vocal interaction. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 93–122). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M. (2003). Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: Cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cognitive Development, 18, 533–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M., & Trevarthen, C. (2008). Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. The Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(10–11), 122–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubley, P., & Trevarthen, C. (1979). Sharing a task in infancy. In I. Uzgiris (Ed.), Social interaction during infancy: New directions for child development (vol. 4, pp. 57–80). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., & Felstein, S. (1970). Rhythms of dialogue. New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lagercrantz, H. (2016). Infant brain development. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Llinás, R. (2001). I of the vortex: From neurons to self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., & Buder, E. H. (1995). Phrasing in prelinguistic vocalisations. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, S. (1999). Mother and infants and communicative musicality. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 29–57). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2010). Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H., Mpodozis, J., & Carlos Letelier, J. (1995). Brain, language and the origin of human mental functions. Biological Research, 28(1), 15–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. (1999). Synchronous chorusing and the origins of music. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 59–74). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, N. (2010). Towards a chronobiology of musical rhythm. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship (pp. 545–564). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2011). Archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Trevarthen, C. (2010). The neuroscience of emotion in music. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship (pp. 105–146). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1981). Musical elements in the infant’s vocalization: Their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. K. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (vol. 1, pp. 163–224). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piontelli, A. (2002). Twins: From fetus to child. London, England: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, and self-regulation. New York, NY: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakic, P. (1991). Development of the primate cerebral cortex. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: A comprehensive textbook (pp. 11–28). Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others’ expectations; teasing and mucking about in the first year. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 143–158). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3(2), 186–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N., & Kisilevsky, B. (Eds.). (2015). Fetal development: Research on brain and behavior, environmental influences, and emerging technologies. London, England: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robb, L. (1999). Emotional musicality in mother-infant vocal affect, and an acoustic study of postnatal depression. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 123–151). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schögler, B. W. (1999). Studying temporal co-ordination in jazz duets. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999-2000 (pp. 75–92). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. (1777/1982). Of the nature of that imitation which takes place in what are called the imitative arts. Republished in W. P. D. Wightman & J. C. Bryce (Eds.), 1982. Essays on philosophical subjects (pp. 176–213). Indianapolis, IN: Liberty Fund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1971). A micro-analysis of mother-infant interaction: Behaviors regulating social contact between a mother and her three-and-a-half-month-old twins. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 10, 501–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2000). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and development psychology. (Second Edition, with new Introduction. Originally published in 1985) New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2004). The present moment: In psychotherapy and everyday life. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2010). Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N., Hofer, L., Haft, W., & Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 249–268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E. (2003). Musical predispositions in infancy: An update. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of music (pp. 3–20). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1974). The psychobiology of speech development. In E. H. Lenneberg (Ed.), Language and brain: Developmental aspects (vol. 12, pp. 570–585). Boston, MA: Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). London, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1990). Signs before speech. In T. A. Sebeok & J. Umiker-Sebeok (Eds.), The semiotic web, 1989 (pp. 689–755). Berlin, Germany/New York, NY/Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 157–213). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2001). The neurobiology of early communication: Intersubjective regulations in human brain development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook on brain and behavior in human development (pp. 841–882). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2005). Stepping away from the mirror: Pride and shame in adventures of companionship reflections on the nature and emotional needs of infant intersubjectivity. In C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossman, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, & N. Sachser (Eds.), Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis, Dahlem workshop report (vol. 92, pp. 55–84). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2009). Human biochronology: On the source and functions of ‘musicality’, chap. 18. In R. Haas & V. Brandes (Eds.), Music that works: Contributions of biology, neurophysiology, psychology, sociology, medicine and musicology (pp. 221–266). Vienna, Austria/New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2015). Infant semiosis: The psycho-biology of action and shared experience from birth. Cognitive Development, 36C, 130–141. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2016). From the intrinsic motive pulse of infant actions, to the life time of cultural meanings. In B. Mölder, V. Arstila, & P. Øhrstrom (Eds.), Philosophy and psychology of time, Springer studies in brain and mind (vol. 9, pp. 225–265). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer International. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-22195-3 ISBN 978-3-319-22195-3

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (1994). Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 599–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., Aitken, K. J., Vandekerckhove, M., Delafield-Butt, J., & Nagy, E. (2006). Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: Stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, volume 2, developmental neuroscience (2nd ed.pp. 65–126). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J. (2015). The infant’s creative vitality, in projects of self-discovery and shared meaning: How they anticipate school, and make it fruitful. In S. Robson & S. F. Quinn (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of young children’s thinking and understanding (pp. 3–18). Abingdon, Oxon/New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J. (2017). The development of consciousness. In B. Hopkins, E. Geangu, & S. Linkenauger (Eds.), Cambridge encyclopedia of child development (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK/New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., Delafield-Butt, J., & Schögler, B. (2011). Psychobiology of musical gesture: Innate rhythm, harmony and melody in movements of narration. In A. Gritten & E. King (Eds.), Music and gesture 2. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2013). Biology of shared experience and language development: Regulations for the inter-subjective life of narratives. In M. Legerstee, D. Haley, & M. Bornstein (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain (pp. 167–199). New York, NY: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Fresquez, C. (2015). Sharing human movement for wellbeing: Research on communication in infancy and applications in dance movement psychotherapy. Body, Movement and Dance in Psychotherapy. doi:10.1080/17432979.2015.1084948

  • Trevarthen, C., Gratier, M. and Osborne, N. (2014, March/April). The human nature of culture and education. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol. 5, 173-192. doi:10.1002/wcs.1276.

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). London, England/New York, NY/San Francisco, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Malloch, S. (2000). The dance of wellbeing: Defining the musical therapeutic effect. The Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 9(2), 3–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Malloch, S. (2002, September). Musicality and music before three: Human vitality and invention shared with pride. Zero to Three, 23(1), 10–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Marwick, H. (1986). Signs of motivation for speech in infants, and the nature of a mother's support for development of language. In B. Lindblom & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 279–308). Basingstoke, Hampshire: Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. and Reddy, V. (2016, in press). Consciousness in infants. In, S. Schneider and M. Velmans (Eds.). Blackwell companion to consciousness, 2nd Edition. Oxford: Blackwells.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Rees, S. and de Leeuw, R. (1993). Born too early: The kangaroo method with premature babies. Heythuysen, The Netherlands: Video by Stichting Lichaamstaal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B Biological Science, 364, 1955–1967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M., & Pöppel, E. (1999). Temporal mechanisms of the brain as fundamentals of communication ì with special reference to music perception and performance. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000 (pp. 13–28). Liège, Belgium: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colwyn Trevarthen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Trevarthen, C. (2017). Maternal Voice and Communicative Musicality: Sharing the Meaning of Life from Before Birth. In: Filippa, M., Kuhn, P., Westrup, B. (eds) Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development . Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65077-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics