Skip to main content

Chinese Thinking Styles and Religion

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Religious Cognition in China

Part of the book series: New Approaches to the Scientific Study of Religion ((NASR,volume 2))

Abstract

Is there any experimental evidence indicating there is something different or distinct about the psychology of ethnic Chinese? Might such differences challenge the purported universality of the cognitive biases outlined in succeeding chapters of this volume? The most developed body of psychological literature showing evidence of significant differences in the reasoning styles of Chinese and “Western” participants is that examining holistic vs. analytic reasoning styles. In this chapter we first introduce this body of research’s key terms and key empirical findings demonstrating Chinese preference for holistic reasoning. Then, we connect particular elements of holistic reasoning to cultural logics operant in cornerstones of traditional Chinese thought—yin-yang theory, Doctrine of the Mean (zhong yong), wu wei, past-oriented reasoning (i.e., assumptions of cyclical time), and pragmatism—that may, hermeneutic scholarship suggests, challenge the universality of cognitive biases posited by the naturalness theory of religion. Finally, in our conclusion we attempt to draw a finer point on particular challenges holistic reasoning may present to particular components of the naturalness theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Brislin, R. W., & Kim, E. S. (2003). Cultural diversity in people’s understanding and uses of time. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52(3), 363–382. doi:10.111/1464-0597.00140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cai, H., Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., Wang, C., Carvallo, M., Xu, Y., et al. (2011). Tactical self-enhancement in China: Is modesty at the service of self-enhancement in East Asian culture? Social Psychology and Personality Science, 2(1), 59–64. doi:10.1177/1948550610376599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, L. H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7(4), 235–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Prycker, V. (2011). Unself-conscious control: Broadening the notion of control through experiences of flow and wu-wei. Zygon, 46(1), 5–25. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9744.2010.01154.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBernardi, J. (2009). Wudang Mountain and Mount Zion in Taiwan: Syncretic processes in space, ritual performance, and imagination. Asian Journal of Social Science, 37(1), 138–162. doi:10.1163/156853109X385420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, L. (2003). Popular religion in contemporary China. Social Compass, 50, 449–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, R. J. (1981). The role of perception of time in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 7(4), 335–342. doi:10.1086/208823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo, T., Ji, L., Spina, R., & Zhang, Z. (2012). Culture, temporal focus, and values of the past and the future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(8), 1030–1040. doi:10.1177/0146167212443895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • He, Q. (2012). Religious traditions in local communities of China. Pastoral Psychology, 61(5), 823–839. doi:10.1007/s11089-012-0438-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, D. F. (1994). Cognitive socialization in Confucian heritage cultures. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 285–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, F. L. K. (1981). Americans and Chinese: Passages to differences (3rd ed.). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J. (2005). Culture and lay theories of change. In R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J. Olson, & M. Zanna (Eds.), Culture and social behavior: The tenth Ontario symposium (pp. 117–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J. (2008). The leopard cannot change his spots, or can he: Culture and the development of lay theories of change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(5), 613–622. doi:10.1177/0146167207313935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Guo, T., Zhang, Z., & Messervey, D. (2009). Looking into the past: Cultural differences in perception and representation of past information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 761–769. doi:10.1037/a0014498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psychological Science, 12(6), 450–456. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 943–955. doi:10.I037//0O22-3514.78.S.943.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Schwarz, N., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, autobiographical memory, and behavioral frequency reports: Measurement issues in cross-cultural studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5), 585–593. doi:10.1177/0146167200267006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z., & Guo, T. (2008). To buy or to sell: Cultural differences in stock market decisions based on price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21(4), 399–413. doi:10.1002/bdm.595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture, or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 57–65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z., Usborne, E., & Guan, Y. (2004). Optimism across cultures: In response to the severe acute respiratory syndrome outbreak. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 7(1), 25–34. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2004.00132.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, X. (2013). Chinese dialectical thinking: The yin-yang model. Philosophy Compass, 8(5), 438–446. doi:10.1111/phc3.12035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lafargue, M. (2001). Nature as part of human culture in Daoism. In N. J. Girardot, J. Miller, & X. Liu (Eds.), Daoism and ecology: Ways within a cosmic landscape (pp. 45–60). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lai, H. H. (2003). The religious revival in China. The Copenhagen Journal of Asian Studies, 18, 40–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, J. D., & Peng, K. (2007). Valuing cultural differences in behavioral economics. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 4(1), 32–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang, L. (2012). Multiple variations: Perspectives on the religious psychology of Buddhist and Christian converts in the People’s Republic of China. Pastoral Psychology, 61(5), 865–877. doi:10.1007/s11089-012-0462-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, T. (1974). On the art of ruling a big country: Views of these Chinese emperors. Canberra, Australia: Australian National University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, I. M. (1986). Chinese cognition. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 73–105). Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X. (1999). An inquiry into the core value of Laozi’s philosophy. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & P. J. Ivanhoe (Eds.), Religious and philosophical aspects of the Laozi (pp. 211–238). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, L. (2001). Understanding happiness: A look into the Chinese folk psychology. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2(4), 407–432. doi:10.1023/A:1013944228205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma-Kellams, C., & Blascovich, J. (2012). Enjoying life in the face of death: East-West differences in responses to mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 773–786. doi:10.1037/a0029366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (2003). Daoism: A short introduction. Oxford, UK: Oneworld Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moneta, G. B. (2004). The flow model of intrinsic motivation in Chinese: Cultural and personal moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5(2), 181–217. doi:10.1023/B:JOHS.0000035916.27782.e4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attribution of physical and social events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mou, Z., & Zhang, J. (2000). General history of Chinese religions. Beijing, China: Religious and Cultural Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, H. (1964). Ways of thinking of Eastern peoples: India, China, Tibet, Japan. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently…and why. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291–310. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Lee, A. (2010). It was meant to happen: Explaining cultural variations in fate attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(5), 702–720. doi:10.1037/a0019141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54(9), 741–754. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollmann, T. (1998). On forgetting the historical past. Memory & Cognition, 26(2), 320–329. doi:10.3758/BF03201143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritsema, R., & Karcher, S. (1994). I Ching: The classic Chinese oracle of change. Dorset, UK: Element Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slingerland, E. (2003). Effortless action: Wu-wei as conceptual metaphor and spiritual ideal in early China. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinks, J. A., Lam, L. M. O., & Van Lingen, G. (1998). Cultural determinants of creativity: An implicit theory approach. In S. Dingli (Ed.), Creative thinking: Towards broader horizons. La Valletta, Malta: Malta University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 242–259. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 288–307. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. L., Louie, J. Y., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children's storybooks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(1), 17–30. doi:10.1177/0146167206292749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Norden, B. W. (1999). Method in the madness of the Laozi. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & P. J. Ivanhoe (Eds.), Religious and philosophical aspects of the Laozi (pp. 187–210). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H. A., & Asch, S. E. (1948). Studies in space orientation. III. Perception of the upright in the absence of a visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(5), 603–614. doi:10.1037/h0055372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W. Y. (2011). Defining Chinese folk religion: A methodological interpretation. Asian Philosophy, 21(2), 153–170. doi:10.1080/09552367.2011.563993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xing, Y., & Sims, D. (2012). Leadership, Daoist wu wei and reflexivity: Flow, self-protection and excuse in Chinese bank managers’ leadership practice. Management Learning, 43(1), 97–112. doi:10.1177/1350507611409659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, X., Yang, Q., Dong, N., & Wang, L. (2010). Moderating effect of Zhong yong on the relationship between creativity and innovative behavior. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 13(1), 53–57. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2010.01300.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeoh, S. G. (2008). For/against hybridity: Religious entrepreneurship in a roman Catholic pilgrimage shrine in Malaysia. Asian Journal of Social Science, 37(1), 7–28. doi:10.1163/156853109X385376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuo, B. (2001). Lines and circles. West and East English Today, 17(3), 3–8. doi:10.1017/S0266078401003017.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Li-Jun Ji .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ji, LJ., Chan, E. (2017). Chinese Thinking Styles and Religion. In: Hornbeck, R., Barrett, J., Kang, M. (eds) Religious Cognition in China. New Approaches to the Scientific Study of Religion , vol 2. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-62954-4_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics