Advertisement

Staying on Topic: Doing Research Between Improvisation and Systematisation

  • Constance de Saint-Laurent
Chapter
Part of the Palgrave Studies in Creativity and Culture book series (PASCC)

Abstract

Doing scientific research is, in theory, a systematic and well-organised enterprise. In reality, however, things often go astray: field works get cancelled, interviews get side-tracked and participants drop out. The investigation of human lives, as it turns out, cannot do away with the messiness of human lives. In such cases, researchers must adapt to the new situation and yet stay on topic: in one word, they need to improvise. How, then, does research remain scientific? What matters is what is done afterwards; how hunches and surprises are turned into systematic investigations, analyses and interpretations. This argument will be illustrated with the story of an ‘impromptu’ fieldwork and its unpredictable consequences; or, rather, how staying on topic requires one to systematically stray away from it.

Keywords

Qualitative interviews Improvisation Systematisation Collective memory 

References

  1. Billig, M. (2013). Learn to write badly. How to succeed in the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brinkmann, S. (2012). Qualitative research between craftsmanship and McDonaldization. A keynote address from the 17th qualitative health research conference. Qualitative Studies, 3(1), 56–68.Google Scholar
  3. Cornish, F., & Gillespie, A. (2009). A pragmatist approach to the problem of knowledge in health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 800–809. doi: 10.1177/1359105309338974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2014). “I would rather be hanged than to agree with you!”: Collective memory and the definition of the nation in parliamentary debates on immigration. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 15, 22–53.Google Scholar
  5. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2015). Dialogue and debate in psychology: Commentary on the foundational myth of psychology as a science. In J. Cresswell, A. Haye, A. Larrain, M. Morgan, & G. Sullivan (Eds.), Dialogue and debate in the making of theoretical psychology (pp. 231–239). Concord: Captus University Publications.Google Scholar
  6. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2017). Personal trajectories, collective memories. Remembering and the life-course. Culture & Psychology 23(2), 263–279.Google Scholar
  7. de Saint-Laurent, C. (in press). Trajectories of resistance and historical reflections. In N. Chaudhary, G. Marsico, P. Hviid, & J. Villadsen (Eds.), Resistance in everyday life: Constructing cultural experiences. Singapore: Springer.Google Scholar
  8. de Saint-Laurent, C. (Submitted-a). Historical reasoning: A cultural psychology perspective on emergence in collective memory. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science.Google Scholar
  9. de Saint-Laurent, C. (Submitted-b). Collective memory beyond memory: Psychosocial processes in historical reasoning.Google Scholar
  10. de Saint-Laurent, C., & Zittoun, T. (in press). Memory in life transitions. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Handbook of memory and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Favero, P. (2010). Italians, the “good people”: Reflections on national self-representation in contemporary Italian debates on xenophobia and war. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 12(2), 138–153.Google Scholar
  12. Glaveanu, V. P. (2014). Thinking through creativity and culture: Toward an integrated model (1st ed.). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  13. James, W. (1922). Pragmatism: A new name for some old ways of thinking. New York/London: Longmans, Green and Co.Google Scholar
  14. Nicholson, C. (2016). The role of historical representations in Israeli-Palestinian relations: Narratives from abroad. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 22(1), 5–11. doi: 10.1037/pac0000143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Pierce, C. S. (1877). The fixation of belief. Popular Science Monthly, 12, 1–15. doi: 10.7135/UPO9780857286512.036.Google Scholar
  16. Rosenstein, A. (2014). Décris-Ravage. Brussels: Théâtre Océan Nord.Google Scholar
  17. Rosenthal, G. (1993). Reconstruction of life stories: Principles of selection in generating stories for narrative biographical interviews. The Narrative Study of Lives, 1(1), 59–91.Google Scholar
  18. Valsiner, J. (2014). Needed for cultural psychology: Methodology in a new key. Culture & Psychology, 20(1), 3–30. doi: 10.1177/1354067X13515941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Wertsch, J. (2002). Voices of collective remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Wertsch, J., & Batiashvili, N. (2012). Mnemonic communities and conflict: Georgia’s narrative template. In I. Marková & A. Gillespie (Eds.), Trust and conflict: Representation, culture and dialogue. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  21. Zittoun, T. (2006). Transitions: Development through symbolic resources. Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  22. Zittoun, T. (2012). Lifecourse: A socio-cultural perspective. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), The handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 513–535). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Constance de Saint-Laurent
    • 1
  1. 1.University of NeuchâtelNeuchâtelSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations