Skip to main content

What Are People’s Lay Theories About Mind Wandering and How Do Those Beliefs Affect Them?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Science of Lay Theories

Abstract

Many of the thoughts that pass through our minds are unrelated to our current activity or environment. The mind simply wanders, often spontaneously and without intention. Over the last couple of decades, much progress has been made in getting a better scientific understanding of the causes and consequences of such spontaneous stimulus-unrelated thoughts, or mind wandering, but only very recently have studies started to examine people’s lay theories about these thoughts. This chapter gives an overview of the current state of this research. First, we discuss lay theories about the frequency and importance of spontaneous stimulus-unrelated thoughts. We then review recent evidence suggesting that the extent to which people believe that their tendency to mind wander is controllable or outside their control affects how much they actually mind wander. We will also draw on clinical research addressing the aspect of controllability in the context of intrusive thoughts, which, while common among healthy individuals, are also a hallmark feature of a range of mental disorders. We examine the role lay theories about intrusive thoughts play in the development and maintenance of these disorders. Finally, we discuss ways in which it may be possible to change people’s lay theories about spontaneous stimulus-unrelated thoughts in adaptive ways.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus-independent-thought. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 423–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Fishman, D. J., Mrazek, M. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Unnoticed intrusions: Dissociations of meta-consciousness in thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1003–1012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 0956797612446024.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1604–1611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J. G., Gudmundsdottir, B., Palyo, S. A., Miller, L. M., & Grant, D. M. (2006). Rebound effects following deliberate thought suppression: Does PTSD make a difference? Behavior Therapy, 37(2), 170–180.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, N. C., Abramowitz, J. S., Pardue, C. M., & Wheaton, M. G. (2010). The relationship between religion and thought–action fusion: Use of an in vivo paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(7), 670–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernsten, A., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2007). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1093–1144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, J. L., & Finkel, E. J. (2012). Buffering against weight gain following dieting setbacks: An implicit theory intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 721–725. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, J. L., O’Boyle, E. H., VanEpps, E. M., Pollack, J. M., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Mind-sets matter: A meta-analytic review of implicit theories and self-regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byers, E. S., Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (1998). Sexual intrusive thoughts of college students. Journal of Sex Research, 35(4), 359–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bywaters, M., Andrade, J., & Turpin, G. (2004). Intrusive and non-intrusive memories in a non-clinical sample: The effects of mood and affect on imagery vividness. Memory, 12(4), 467–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, R. A., & Guthrie, R. M. (2005). Maladaptive appraisals as a risk factor for posttraumatic stress. Psychological Science, 16, 749–752. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01608

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, P., & Giuntoli, D. (1972). Consciousness sampling in the college classroom or is anybody listening? Intellect, 101, 63–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, E., Kofler, L., Forster, D., & McCullough, M. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.1037/xge0000083

  • Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 813–838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A. (2005). Intrusive thoughts in clinical disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & de Silva, P. (1985). The nature of depressive and anxious, intrusive thoughts: Distinct or uniform phenomena? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(4), 383–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & Purdon, C. (2009). Mental control of unwanted intrusive thoughts: A phenomenological study of nonclinical individuals. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2(3), 267–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., Purdon, C., & Byers, E. S. (2000). Appraisal and control of sexual and non-sexual intrusive thoughts in university students. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(5), 439–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., & Rhyno, S. (2005). Unwanted intrusive thoughts in nonclinical individuals: Implications for clinical disorders. In D. A. Clark (Ed.), Unwanted intrusive thoughts in clinical disorders. New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, P. (1991). The Development of defense mechanisms: Theory, research, and assessment. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. (1987). A folk model of the mind. In D. Holland & N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 112–148). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, M. I., & Clark, D. M. (1998). Thought suppression produces a rebound effect with analogue post-traumatic intrusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(6), 571–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M. T., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Depletion makes the heart grow less helpful: Helping as a function of self-regulatory energy and genetic relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(12), 1653–1662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 69–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry: An International Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory, 6, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, P. L., & Wells, A. (2005). Experimental modification of beliefs in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A test of the metacognitive model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(6), 821–829.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., & Liberman, N. (2001). The role of attribution of motivation in producing postsuppressional rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., & Liberman, N. (2004). A motivational model of post-suppressional rebound. European review of social psychology, 15(1), 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Anderson, C. L., Johnston, C., Smallwood, J., Kingstone, A., et al. (2014). Tracking distraction: The relationship between mind-wandering, meta-awareness, and ADHD symptomatology. Journal of Attention Disorders. doi:10.1177/1087054714543494

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Anderson, C. L., Smallwood, J., Kingstone, A., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The silver lining of a mind in the clouds: Interesting musings are associated with positive mood while mind-wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, e583. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(4), 303–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., Brewer, L. E., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gailliot, M. T., Peruche, B. M., Plant, E. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Stereotypes and prejudice in the blood: Sucrose drinks reduce prejudice and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 288–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galera, C., Orriols, L., M’Bailara, K., Laborey, M., Contrand, B., Ribereau-Gayon, R., et al. (2012). Mindwandering and driving: Responsibility case-control study. British Medical Journal, 345, e8105. doi:10.1136/bmj.e8105

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1989). Task-unrelated thought frequency as a function of age: A laboratory study. Psychology of Aging, 4, 136–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzaga, G. C., Haselton, M. G., Smurda, J., Davies, M. S., & Poore, J. C. (2008). Love, desire, and the suppression of thoughts of romantic alternatives. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(2), 119–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grodsky, A., & Giambra, L. M. (1990). The consistency across vigilance and reading tasks of individual differences in the occurrence of task-unrelated and task-related images and thoughts. Imagination, Cognition & Personality, 10, 39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, S. E., & Tolin, D. F. (2005). Mindfulness-and acceptance-based behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. In Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches to anxiety (pp. 271–299). Springer US.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, J. A., & Blankenship, V. (2002). Ruminative thoughts and their relation to depression and anxiety. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(3), 465–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A. G. (2002). A cognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(8), 869–893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A. G., & Payne, S. (2002). The management of unwanted pre-sleep thoughts in insomnia: Distraction with imagery versus general distraction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(3), 267–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, D. (1968). Dimensions of projection. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 248–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iijima, Y., & Tanno, Y. (2012). The rebound effect in the unsuccessful suppression of worrisome thoughts. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(3), 347–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1992). A process-dissociation framework for investigating unconscious influences: Freudian slips, projective tests, subliminal perception, and signal detection theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(6), 174–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, S. G. (2012). Mindfulness training affects attention—Or is it attentional effort? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 106–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jha, A. P., Morrison, A. B., Dainer-Best, J., Parker, S., Rostrup, N., & Stanley, E. (2015). Minds “at Attention”: Mindfulness training curbs attentional lapses in military cohorts. PLoS ONE, 10, e0116889.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Beliefs about willpower determine the impact of glucose on self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 14837–14842.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Beliefs about willpower predict self-regulation and grades in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 637–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: Thought flow and motivation. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 225–239). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (2013). Goal commitments and the content of thoughts and dreams: Basic principles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, e415. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E., & Cox, W. M. (1987). Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 7(2), 105–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Rassin, E., Crombez, G., & Näring, G. W. (2003). The paradoxical effects of suppressing anxious thoughts during imminent threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(9), 1113–1120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A. E., Hu, Y., Magee, J. C., Beadel, J. R., & Teachman, B. A. (2014). Thought suppression across time: Change in frequency and duration of thought recurrence. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and related Disorders, 3(1), 21–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012). The persistence of thought: Evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychological Science, 23, 357–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logel, C., Iserman, E. C., Davies, P. G., Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2009). The perils of double consciousness: The role of thought suppression in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 299–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magee, J. C., Harden, K. P., & Teachman, B. A. (2012). Psychopathology and thought suppression: A quantitative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(3), 189–201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Magee, J. C., & Teachman, B. A. (2007). Why did the white bear return? Obsessive–compulsive symptoms and attributions for unsuccessful thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 2884–2898.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Malle, B., & Knobe, J. (1997). Which behaviors do people explain? A basic actor-observer asymmetry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 288–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcks, B. A., & Woods, D. W. (2005). A comparison of thought suppression to an acceptance-based technique in the management of personal intrusive thoughts: A controlled evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(4), 433–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcks, B. A., & Woods, D. W. (2007). Role of thought-related beliefs and coping strategies in the escalation of intrusive thoughts: An analog to obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2640–2651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martijn, C., Tenbült, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E., & de Vries, N. K. (2002). Getting a grip on ourselves: Challenging expectations about loss of energy after self-control. Social Cognition, 20, 441–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind-wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2009). Tracking the train of thought from the laboratory into everyday life: An experience-sampling study of mind wandering across controlled and ecological contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 857–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 6, 192–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mooneyham, B. W., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: A review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 67, 11–18. doi:10.1037/a0031569

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morewedge, C. K., Giblin, C. E., & Norton, M. I. (2014). The (perceived) meaning of spontaneous thoughts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 1742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morsella, E., Ben-Zeev, A., Lanska, M., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). The spontaneous thoughts of the night: How future tasks breed intrusive cognitions. Social Cognition, 28(5), 641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, A. J., Ihm, E., Molden, D. M., Mrazek, M. D., Zedelius, C., & Schooler, J. W. (2017). Growth mindsets of self-control and the role of effort. Preliminary data, University of California, Santa Barbara

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24, 774–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12, 442–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 147–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. A. (1951). Uses of the thematic apperception test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 107(8), 577–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Najmi, S., Riemann, B. C., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Managing unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive—compulsive disorder: Relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 494–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2005). Psychometric validation of the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusions inventory—Part 2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(11), 1527–1542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, G., Chard, K., & Cox, T. (2008). The relationship between maladaptive cognitions, anger expression, and posttraumatic stress disorder among veterans in residential treatment. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 17, 439–452. doi:10.1080/10926770802473908

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2005). Violations of implicit theories and the sense of prediction and control: Implications for motivated person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2009). Lay theories of Personality: Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social and Personality Compass, 3, 1069–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, D. A., Lindsay, D. S., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1995). Psychotherapy and the recovery of memories of childhood sexual abuse: US and British practitioners’ opinions, practices, and experiences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63(3), 426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Tang, Y.-Y. (2015). Enhancing attention through training. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 4, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological Review, 111(3), 781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (2002). The need to control thoughts. In R. O. Frost & G. Steketee (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, assessment, and treatment. London: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1981). Part I. Unwanted intrusive cognitions. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 3(3), 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793–802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16(4), 233–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radomsky, A. S., Alcolado, G. M., Abramowitz, J. S., Alonso, P., Belloch, A., Bouvard, M., et al. (2014). Part 1—You can run but you can’t hide: Intrusive thoughts on six continents. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3(3), 269–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reuven-Magril, O., Rosenman, M., Liberman, N., & Dar, R. (2009). Manipulating meta-cognitive beliefs about the difficulty to suppress scratching: Implications for obsessive-compulsive disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2(2), 143–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, M., & Brewin, C. R. (1999). Intrusive memories in depression and posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(3), 201–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, M., & Salkovskis, P. M. (1992). Comparison of positive and negative intrusive thoughts and experimental investigation of the differential effects of mood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(3), 273–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risko, E. F., Anderson, N., Sarwal, A., Engelhardt, M., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Everyday attention: Variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 234–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rummel, J., & Boywitt, C. D. (2014). Controlling the stream of thought: Working memory capacity predicts adjustment of mind wandering to situational demands. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1309–1315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G., Pierce, G. R., & Sarason, B. R. (1996). Domains of cognitive interference. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. 139–152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Enhancing attention and memory during video-recorded lectures. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 1(1), 60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 339–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Baird, B., Mooneyham, B. W., Zedelius, C., et al. (2014). The middle way: Finding the balance between mindfulness and mind-wandering. Psychology of learning and motivation, 60, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Reichele, E., & Halpern, D. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences, 15, 319–326. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006

    Google Scholar 

  • Scruton, R. (1982). Kant. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., et al. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1139–1171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2014). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research, e617. doi:10.1007/s00436-014-0617-x

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., & Rachman, S. (2004). Thought-action fusion: A review. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 87–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., & Rachman, S. (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10, 379–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L., & Schonbar, R. A. (1961). Correlates of daydreaming: A dimension of self-awareness. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 25, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J. (2010). Why the global availability of mind-wandering necessitates resource competition: Reply to McVay and Kane (2010). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 202–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M. C., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 527–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1144–1150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., Nind, L., & O’Connor, R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 118–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M., & Reid, H. (2003). The effects of block duration and task demands on the experience of task unrelated thought. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 22, 13–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, R. (2004). Does ‘ought’ imply ‘can’? And did Kant think it does? Utilitas, 16(01), 42–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y.-Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 222–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vranas, P. B. (2007). I ought, therefore I can. Philosophical Studies, 136(2), 167–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Seli, P., Allan, J., Boucher, P. O., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures II: Relation to academic performance. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2(1), 33–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (2011). Setting free the bears: Escape from thought suppression. American Psychologist, 66(8), 671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Gold, D. (1995). Fanning old flames: Emotional and cognitive effects of suppressing thoughts of a past relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 782–792.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Changing our minds: An introduction to mental control. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 1–12). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Smart, L. (1997). Deep cognitive activation: A new approach to the unconscious. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 984–995.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A. (2005). Detached mindfulness in cognitive therapy: A metacognitive analysis and ten techniques. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 23(4), 337–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 59–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M., & Roper, D. W. (1988). Depression and mental control: The resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(6), 882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicklow, A., & Espie, C. A. (2000). Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to actigraphic measurement of sleep: Towards a cognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(7), 679–693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson-Tough, M., Bocci, L., Thorne, K., & Herlihy, J. (2010). Is mindfulness-based therapy an effective intervention for obsessive–intrusive thoughts: A case series. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 17(3), 250–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, E. F., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Do people really believe they are above average? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1121–1128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpe, J. (1990). The practice of behavioral therapy (4th ed.). New York, NY: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47, 301–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). Motivating meta-awareness of mind wandering: A way to catch the mind in flight? Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 44–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., Protzko, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2017a). Below-average lost in thoughts: People believe their own mind wandering rates to be lower than those of other people. Preliminary data, University of California, Santa Barbara.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., Protzko, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2017b). Lay theories of mind wandering affect the rate of mind wandering in everyday life and in the lab. Preliminary data, University of California, Santa Barbara.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). Mind wandering “Ahas” versus mindful reasoning: Alternative routes to creative solutions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claire M. Zedelius .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zedelius, C.M., Schooler, J.W. (2017). What Are People’s Lay Theories About Mind Wandering and How Do Those Beliefs Affect Them?. In: Zedelius, C., Müller, B., Schooler, J. (eds) The Science of Lay Theories. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57306-9_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics