Skip to main content

The Motivated Fluidity of Lay Theories of Change

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Science of Lay Theories

Abstract

People vary in their chronic beliefs about the malleability of human attributes such as intelligence and morality. These lay theories of change can play a powerful role in people’s self-regulation, person perception, and decisions. Although we acknowledge that people may have a lay theory to which they predominantly turn, we consider the ways that people may actively shift their endorsement of lay theories depending on the context and their goals. Drawing on a motivated reasoning perspective, we describe how people sometimes selectively gravitate toward a lay theory of change or stability depending on which viewpoint allows them to support their desired position or to satisfy an underlying motivation. We consider how people’s goals may influence their selective preference for lay theories in the context of personal, social, intergroup, and societal-level judgments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Once a thief, always a thief; a leopard cannot change its spots.

  2. 2.

    However, it is also possible that people do not even hold the same implicit theories about self and others, although the general person scale seems able to predict both personal and other judgments. Some recent research developed a self-theories version of the implicit theories scale based on the recognition that people might have one belief about how malleable intelligence is in general, and a different view of their own personal intelligence. On average, people reported that they themselves were more malleable than others, and self-theories were a better predictor of students’ own personal academic motivation and responses (De Castella & Byrne, 2015). Likewise, Aneeta Rattan and colleagues demonstrated that people may not apply the same theory of mutability to all people or groups. People who believe that the capacity for improvement is universal are more likely to support policies that promote equal opportunity, while those who believe that only some people have the capacity to become highly intelligent are less inclined to support such measures (Rattan & Georgeac, this volume; Rattan, Savani, Naidu, & Dweck, 2012).

References

  • ABC News. (2016, October 9). Transcript of this week with George Stephanopoulos. Retrieved from http://abcnews.go.com/Politics/week-transcript-rudy-giuliani-donna-brazile/story?id=42670926

  • Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680–740). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradfield, J. P., Taal, H. R., Timpson, N. J., et al. (2012). A genome-wide association meta-analysis identifies new childhood obesity loci. Nature Genetics, 44, 526–531.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brescoll, V. L., & LaFrance, M. (2004). The correlates and consequences of newspaper reports of research on gender differences. Psychological Science, 15, 515–521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brescoll, V. L., Uhlmann, E. L., & Newman, G. E. (2013). The effects of system-justifying motivations on endorsement of essentialist explanations for gender differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 891–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, P. R. (2008). Value war: Public opinion and the politics of gay rights. New York: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, J. L., & Finkel, E. J. (2012). Buffering against weight gain following dieting setbacks: An implicit theory intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 721–725. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, J. L., Pollack, J. M., & Hoyt, C. L. (2010). Individual differences in implicit theories of leadership ability and self-efficacy: Predicting responses to stereotype threat. Journal of Leadership Studies, 3, 46–56. doi:10.1002/jls.20138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3, 23–43. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997a). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.19

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C. Y., Dweck, C. S., Tong, J. Y., & Fu, J. H. (1997b). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 923–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clance, P. R., & Imes, S. A. (1978). The imposter phenomenon in high achieving women: Dynamics and therapeutic intervention. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 15, 241–247. doi:10.1037/h0086006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidai, S., & Gilovich, T. (2015). Building a more mobile America—One income quintile at a time. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 60–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. (2011). Genetic essentialism: on the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 800–818.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Day, M. V. & Fiske, S. T. (2016). Movin’ on up? How perceptions of social mobility affect our willingness to defend the system. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 267–274. doi:10.1177/1948550616678454.

  • De Castella, K., & Byrne, D. (2015). My intelligence may be more malleable than yours: the revised implicit theories of intelligence (self-theory) scale is a better predictor of achievement, motivation, and student disengagement. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 30, 245–267. doi:10.1007/s10212-015-0244-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du Monteil, E. (2015). 20 reasons to believe in the American Dream. XPat Nation. http://xpatnation.com/reasons-to-believe-in-the-american-dream/

  • Dweck, C. S. (2012). Implicit theories. In P. Van Lange, A. Wruglanski, & E. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 43–62). London: SAGE Publications Limited.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995a). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0604_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995b). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322–333. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0604_12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644–656. doi:10.1177/0146167293195015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervey, B. M., Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Differential use of person information in decisions about guilt versus innocence: The role of implicit theories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 17–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. (2013). David and Goliath: Underdogs, misfits, and the art of battling giants (1st ed.). New York, NY: Little, Brown and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development, 84, 1526–1541. doi:10.1111/cdev.12064

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, J. (2006). The great risk shift: The new economic insecurity and the decline of the American dream. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, J., & Pierson, P. (2010). Winner-take-all politics: How Washington made the rich richer and turned its back on the middle class. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hafer, C. L., & Bègue, L. (2005). Experimental research on just-world theory: Problems, developments, and future challenges. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 128–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. A., Vernon, P. A., Olson, J. M., & Jang, K. L. (1999). Self-rated personality and intelligence: A multivariate genetic analysis. European Journal of Personality, 13, 121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N. (2017). The origins of lay theories: The case of essentialist beliefs. In C. M. Zedelius, B. C. N. Müller, & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), The science of lay theories: How beliefs shape our cognition, behavior, and health. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, P., & Golden, A. M. (2008). Attributional beliefs about the controllability of stigmatized traits: Antecedents or justifications of prejudice? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 1023–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.588

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y., Levy, S. R., & Chiu, C. (2001). The contribution of the lay theories approach to the study of groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 98–106. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0502_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iatridis, T., & Fousiani, K. (2009). Effects of status and outcome on attributions and just-world beliefs: How the social distribution of success and failure may be rationalized. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 415–420. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jayaratne, T. E., et al. (2006). White Americans’ genetic lay theories of race differences and sexual orientation: Their relationship with prejudice toward blacks, and gay men and lesbians. Group Processes Intergroup Relations, 9, 77–94. doi:10.1177/1368430206059863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 200–206. doi:10.1177/014616727800400205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Sheldon, O., & Sullivan, B. N. (2003). Social inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the system: Evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvantaged. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 13–36. doi:10.1002/ejsp.127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kammrath, L., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Voicing conflict: Preferred conflict strategies among incremental and entity theorists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1497–1508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, A. C., Jost, J. T., & Young, S. (2005). Victim derogation and victim enhancement as alternate routes to system justification. Psychological Science, 16, 240–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, L. E. (1955). Consistency of the adult personality. American Psychologist, 10, 659–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, A. M., & Garavan, H. (2005). Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1089–1102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knowles, E. D., & Ditto, P. H. (2012). Preference, principle, and political casuistry. In J. Hanson (Ed.), Ideology, psychology, and law (pp. 341–379). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krakovsky, M. (2007). The effort effect. Stanford Magazine. https://alumni.stanford.edu/get/page/magazine/article/?article_id=32124

  • Kraus, M. W., Davidai, S., & Nussbaum, A. D. (2015, May 3). American dream? Or mirage? The New York Times.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2013). Social class rank, essentialism, and punitive judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 247–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, M. W., & Tan, J. J. X. (2015). Americans overestimate social class mobility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kumar, S., & Jagacinski, C. M. (2006). Imposters have goals too: The imposter phenomenon and its relationship to achievement goal theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 147–157. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 636–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, C. A. (2016). How three narratives of modernity justify economic inequality. Acta Sociologica, 59, 93–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (2006). Lay theories and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9, 5–24. doi:10.1177/1368430206059855

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1421–1436. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1421

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leith, S. A., Ward, C. L. P., Giacomin, M., Landau, E. S., Ehrlinger, J., & Wilson, A. E. (2014). Changing theories of change: Strategies shifting in implicit theories endorsement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 597–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, G. B. (2009). Does believing homosexuality is innate increase support for gay rights? The Policy Studies Journal, 37, 669–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lou, N. M., & Noels, K. A. (2016). Changing language mindsets: Implications for goal orientations and responses to failure in and outside the second language classroom. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 46, 22–23. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.03.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malahy, L. W., Rubinlicht, M. A., & Kaiser, C. R. (2009). Justifying inequality: A cross-temporal investigation of U.S. income disparities and just-world beliefs from 1973 to 2006. Social Justice Research, 22, 369–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manza, J., & Brooks, C. (2016). Prisoners of the American Dream? Americans’ Attitudes Towards Inequality and Taxes in the New Gilded Age. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.3.192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molden, D. C., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). “Meaningful” social inferences: Effects of implicit theories on inferential processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 738–752. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morin-Chassé, A., Suhay, E., & Jayaratne, T. (2014). Discord over DNA: Politically contingent responses to scientific research on genes and race. APSA 2014 Annual Meeting Paper; American University School of Public Affairs Research Paper No. 2014-0002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, T. A., Hornsey, J. M., & Postmes, T. (2009a). Shifting ground: The variable use of essentialism in contexts of inclusion and exclusion. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 35–59. doi:10.1348/014466607X270287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, T. A., Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., & Hornsey, M. J. (2009b). Theorizing gender in the face of social change: Is there anything essential about essentialism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 653–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness versus remediation: Self-theories and modes of self-esteem maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 599–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obama, B. (2012). State of the Union Address.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peetz, J., & Wilson, A. E. (2008). The temporally extended self: The relation of past and future selves to current identity, motivation, and goal pursuit. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 2090–2106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peetz, J., & Wilson, A. E. (2014). Marking time: Selective use of temporal landmarks as barriers between current and future selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 44–56. doi:10.1177/0146167213501559

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piketty, T. (2014). Capital in the twenty-first century. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2009). Lay theories of personality: Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 1069–1081. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00222.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., & Stecher, K. (2007). Unexpected improvement, decline, and stasis: A prediction confidence perspective on achievement success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 667–684. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.667

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poon, C. S. K., & Koehler, D. J. (2006). Lay personality knowledge and dispositionist thinking: A knowledge-activation framework. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 177–191. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poon, C. S. K., & Koehler, D. J. (2008). Person theories: Their temporal stability and relation to intertrait inferences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 965–977. doi:10.1177/0146167208316690

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.122

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2010). Who confronts prejudice? The role of implicit theories in the motivation to confront prejudice. Psychological Science, 21, 952–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rattan, A., & Georgeac, O. (2017). Mindsets about malleability and intergroup relations. In C. M. Zedelius, B. C. N. Mülle, & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), The science of lay theories: How beliefs shape our cognition, behavior, and health. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattan, A., Savani, K., Naidu, N. V. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Can everyone become highly intelligent? Cultural differences in and societal consequences of beliefs about the universal potential for intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 787–803. doi:10.1037/a0029263

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T. (2008). Reinvigorating the concept of situation in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 311–329. doi:10.1177/1088868308321721

  • Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 792–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasaki, J. Y., LeClair, J., West, A. L., & Kim, H. S. (2016). The gene-culture interaction framework and implications for health. In J. Y. Chiao, S. Li, R. Seligman, & R. Turner (Eds.), The oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience (p. 279). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumaker, J. A., & Slep, A. M. S. (2004). Attitudes and dating aggression: A cognitive dissonance approach. Prevention Science, 5, 231–243. doi:10.1023/B:PREV.0000045357.19100.77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 317–338. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Wiwad, D., & Aknin, L. B. (2016). Income mobility breeds tolerance for income inequality: Cross-national and experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 373–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepperd, J., Malone, W., & Sweeny, K. (2008). Exploring causes of the self-serving bias. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 895–908. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00078.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steimer, A., & Mata, A. (2016). Motivated implicit theories of personality: My weaknesses will go away, but my strengths are here to stay. Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 415–429. doi:10.1177/0146167216629437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Mullen, E. (2008). Morality and justice: An expanded theoretical perspective and empirical review. In K. A. Hegtvedt & J. Clay-Warner (Eds.), Advances in group processes: Justice.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suhay, E., & Garretson, J. (2015, March). Science, sexuality, and civil rights: Does research on the causes of homosexuality have a political impact? 8th Annual NYU-CESS Experimental Political Science Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suhay, E., & Jayaratne, T. E. (2013). Does biology justify ideology? The politics of genetic attribution. Public Opinion Quarterly, 77, 497–521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A. (2001). On the plasticity of self-defense. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 66–69. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1130. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C., & Wilson, A. E. (2015). Implicit theories of change and stability moderate effects of subjective distance on the remembered self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1167–1179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, S. L., & Wilson, A. E. (2016, January). Once a thief, always a thief? How time, implicit theories, and race affect moral judgments. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Diego, California, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., Giordana-Beech, M., Taylor, K. L., Michela, J. L., & Gaus, V. (1994). Strategies of social comparison among people with low self-esteem: Self-protection and self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 713–731. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.713

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47, 302–314. doi:10.1080/00461520.2012.722805

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). An implicit theories of personality intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion. Child Development, 84, 970–988. doi:10.1111/cdev.12003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, A., & Stoeger, H. (2010). A learning oriented subjective action space as an indicator of giftedness. Psychological Science Quarterly, 50, 222–236.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne E. Wilson .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wilson, A.E., English, J.A. (2017). The Motivated Fluidity of Lay Theories of Change. In: Zedelius, C., Müller, B., Schooler, J. (eds) The Science of Lay Theories. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57306-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics