Skip to main content

Technologies to Access Space Without Vision. Some Empirical Facts and Guiding Theoretical Principles

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mobility of Visually Impaired People

Abstract

A large number of technical devices attempt to help blind persons improve their spatial perception and facilitate their mobility . We wish to present here the principles on which these prosthetic perceptual devices function, the conditions of their appropriation, and the general perspectives they open concerning the role of technical objects and systems in the constitution of human experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    “The content of the perceptual state formed in response to a particular pattern of stimulation—the brain’s operative ‘hypothesis’ about the structure of the impinging environment—is the cause to which the highest probability is assigned given all the available endogenous and exogenous evidence. In the case of vision, this will normally be one of indefinitely many possible three-dimensional scenes.” (Briscoe, forthcoming: [10:6]).

  2. 2.

    But, as Heidegger insists: “It requires a very artificial and complicated frame of mind to 'hear' a 'pure noise. […] In the explicit hearing of the discourse of the other, too, we initially understand what is said […]. Even when speaking is unclear or the language is foreign, we initially hear unintelligible words, and not a multiplicity of tone data.” (Heidegger 1927, §.34, p. 153 [p. 164]).

  3. 3.

    Incidentally, the concept of “pointing” used by Siegle and Warren actually presupposes already a spatial framework (if the subject thinks of her action in terms of the gesture of pointing in this or that direction, this means that she already has the experience of a space). This being so, the process whereby this framework is set up is precisely what we are trying to understand here: what is the process of setting up this framework which subsequently makes it possible to interpret gestures as gestures of pointing?

  4. 4.

    Epstein et al. (1986) have studied, in very controlled conditions, the question of the awareness of the existence of an external space through the use of a sensory substitution device—a question we considered again in Auvray et al. [1].

  5. 5.

    The notion of “distal” implies at the same time the idea of “aspect” and of “perspective” on the object: the distal perception of an object, precisely because of the possibility of having access to the latter from an infinity of possible positions, is the perception of the object “under a given aspect”: the aspect that the object presents as “seen from here”.

References

  1. Auvray M, Hanneton S, Lenay C, Kevin O’Regan (2005) There is something out there: distal attribution in sensory substitution, twenty years later. J Integr Neurosci 4(04):505–521

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Auvray M, Hanneton S, O’Regan JK (2007) Learning to perceive with a visuo-auditory substitution system: localisation and object recognition with ‘the voice’. Perception 36(3):416–430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Auvray M, Myin E (2009) Perception with compensatory devices: from sensory substitution to sensorimotor extension. Cogn Sci 33(6):1036–1058

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Bach-y Rita P, Collins CC, Saunders F, White B, Scadden L (1969) Vision substitution by tactile image projection. Nature 221:963–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bach-y-Rita P (1972) Brain mechanisms in sensory substitution. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  6. Bach-y-Rita P, Kercel SW (2003) Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends Cogn Neurosci 7(12):541–546

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bach-y-Rita P, Kaczmarek KA, Tyler ME (2003) A tongue-based tactile display for portrayal of environmental characteristics. Virtual and adaptive environments, 169–186

    Google Scholar 

  8. Bach-y-Rita P (1984) The relationship between motor processes and cognition in tactile vision substitution. In: Cognition and motor processes. Springer, pp 149–160

    Google Scholar 

  9. Bach-y-Rita P (2004) Tactile sensory substitution studies. Ann New York Acad Sci 1013:83–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Briscoe, R (2017) Bodily action and distal attribution in sensory substitution. In: Macpherson F (ed) Sensory substitution and augmentation. Proceedings of the British Academy (forthcoming) Consulté le 15 mars 2016. http://philpapers.org/rec/BRIBAA

  11. Brooks R (1999) Cambrian intelligence. MIT Press, The Early History of the New AI

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  12. Cabe PA, Wright CD, Wright MA (2002) Descartes’s blind man revisited: bimanual triangulation of distance using static hand-held rods. Am J Psychol 116(1):71-98

    Google Scholar 

  13. Collins CC, Bach-y-Rita P (1973) Transmission of pictorial information through the skin. Adv Biol Med Phys 14(1973):285–315

    Google Scholar 

  14. Ditchburn RW (1973) Eye-movements and visual perception. Clarendon, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  15. Epstein W, Hughes B, Schneider S, Bach-y-Rita P (1986) Is there anything out there?: a study of distal attribution in response to vibrotactile stimulation. Perception 15(3):275–84

    Google Scholar 

  16. Fodor JA, Pylyshyn ZW (1981) How direct is visual perception? Some reflections on Gibson’s ‘ecological approach’. Cognition 9(2):139–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gapenne O, Lenay C, Stewart J, Bériot H, Meidine D (2001) Prosthetic device and 2D form perception: the role of increasing degrees of parallelism. In: Proceedings of the conference on assistive technology for vision and hearing impairment (CVHI’2001), pp 113–18

    Google Scholar 

  18. Gapenne O, Rovira K, Lenay C, Stewart J, Auvray M (2005) Is form perception necessary tied to specific sensory feedback?. In: Proceedings, 16. Monterey, CA

    Google Scholar 

  19. Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, Oxford, p 1966

    Google Scholar 

  20. Gibson JJ (2014) The ecological approach to visual perception: classic edition. Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  21. Gregory RL (1970) The intelligent eye. Weidenfeld and Nicolson, London

    Google Scholar 

  22. Gregory RL (1980) Perceptions as hypotheses. Philos Trans Roy Soc Lond B Biol Sci 290(1038):181–197

    Google Scholar 

  23. Grice HP (1962) Some remarks about the senses. In: Butler RJ (ed) Analytical philosophy (first series). Basil Blackwell, Oxford, pp 248–268

    Google Scholar 

  24. Guarniero G (1977) Tactile vision: a personal view. J Vis Impairment Blindness 71(3):125–130

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hanneton S, Auvray M, Durette B (2010) The vibe: a versatile vision-to-audition sensory substitution device. Appl Bion Biomech 7(4):269–276

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hanneton S, Gapenne O, Genouel C, Lenay C, Marque C (1999) Dynamics of shape recognition through a minimal visuo-tactile sensory substitution interface. In: Third international conference on cognitive and neural systems

    Google Scholar 

  27. Kant I (1781) Critique of pure reason. Translated by P. Guyer & A.W. Wood. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1998

    Google Scholar 

  28. Lenay C, Canu S, Villon P (1997) Technology and perception: the contribution of sensory substitution systems. Cognitive technology, Los Alamitos, CA. IEEE Computer Society, USA, pp 44–53

    Google Scholar 

  29. Lenay C, Gapenne O, Hanneton S, Marque C, Genouëlle C (2003) Sensory substitution: limits and perspectives. In: Hatwell Y, Streri A, Gentaz E (eds) Touching for knowing, cognitive psychology of haptic manual perception. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam/Philadelphia, pp 275–92

    Google Scholar 

  30. Lenay C, Thouvenin I, Guénand A, Gapenne O, Stewart J, Maillet B (2007) Designing the ground for pleasurable experience. In: Proceedings of the 2007 conference on Designing pleasurable products and interfaces. ACM, New York, pp 35–58

    Google Scholar 

  31. Lenay C, Steiner P (2010) Beyond the internalism/externalism debate: the constitution of the space of perception. Conscious Cogn 19(4):938–952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Lenay C (2012) Separability and technical constitution. Foundations of science (FOS) Special issue opening up the in-between: interdisciplinary reflections on science, technology and social change, vol 4, pp 379–84. doi:10.1007/s10699-011-9245-8

  33. Loomis JM (1992) Distal attribution and presence. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ 1(1):113–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Meijer PB (1992) An experimental system for auditory image representations. Biomedical Eng IEEE Trans 39(2):112–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Merleau-Ponty M (1945) Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris: Gallimard, Phenomenology of Perception trans. by Colin Smith (New York: Humanities Press, and London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1962); trans. revised by Forrest Williams (1981; reprinted, 2002); new trans. by Donald A. Landes. Routledge, New York, 2012

    Google Scholar 

  36. Noë A (2004) Action in perception. MIT press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  37. O’Regan JK, Noë A (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 24(05):939–973

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Pacherie E (1997) Du problème de Molyneux au problème de Bach-y-Rita. Perception et intermodalite, approches actuelles du probleme de molyneux, pp 255–93

    Google Scholar 

  39. Paillard J (1971) Les déterminants moteurs de l’organisation de l’espace. Cahiers de Psychologie 14(4):261–316

    Google Scholar 

  40. Philipona D, O’Regan JK, Nadal JP (2003) Is there something out there? Inferring space from sensorimotor dependencies. Neural Comput 15(9):2029–2049

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  41. Piaget J (1936) The origins of intelligence in children. Traduit par Margaret Cook et W. W. Norton. vol 8. 5. International Universities Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  42. Segond H, Weiss D, Sampaio E (2005) Human spatial navigation via a visuo-tactile sensory substitution system. Perception 34(10):1231–1249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Siegle JH, Warren WH (2010) Distal attribution and distance perception in sensory substitution. Perception 39(2):208–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Sribunruangrit N, Marque C, Lenay C, Gapenne O, Vanhoutte C (2002) Braille box: analysis of the parallelism concept to access graphic information for blind people. In: Engineering in medicine and biology, 2002. 24th annual conference and the annual fall meeting of the biomedical engineering society EMBS/BMES conference, 2002. Proceedings of the second joint, vol 3, pp 2424–25

    Google Scholar 

  45. Sribunruangrit N, Marque CK, Lenay C, Hanneton S, Gapenne O, Vanhoutte C (2004) Speed-accuracy tradeoff during performance of a tracking task without visual feedback. Neural Syst Rehabilitation Eng IEEE Trans on 12(1):131–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Turvey MT, Shaw RE (1979) The primacy of perceiving: an ecological reformulation of perception for understanding memory. In: Wilsson LG (ed) Perspectives on memory research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 167–222

    Google Scholar 

  47. Varela FJ (1979) Principles of biological autonomy. The North Holland series in general systems research 2. Elsevier North-Holland, Inc., New York, 1979

    Google Scholar 

  48. Wall SA, Brewster S (2006) Sensory substitution using tactile pin arrays: human factors, technology and applications. Sig Process 86(12):3674–95

    Google Scholar 

  49. White BW, Saunders FA, Scadden L, Bach-Y-Rita P, Collins CC (1970) Seeing with the skin. Percept Psychophys 7(1):23–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Ziat M, Lenay C, Gapenne O, Stewart J, Ammar AA, Aubert D (2007) Perceptive supplementation for an access to graphical interfaces. In: Universal access in human computer interaction. Coping with diversity. Springer, New York, pp 841–50

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Lenay .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lenay, C., Declerck, G. (2018). Technologies to Access Space Without Vision. Some Empirical Facts and Guiding Theoretical Principles. In: Pissaloux, E., Velazquez, R. (eds) Mobility of Visually Impaired People. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-54446-5_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-54446-5_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-54444-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-54446-5

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics