Skip to main content

Self-Repair Timing of Lexical Problem Sources: A Window into Primary Language Impairment Online Processing

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 14))

Abstract

Primary Language Impairment (PLI) is a disorder involving both cognitive and linguistic aspects. Indexes of this impairment emerge along conversation when PLI children experience lexical access and production problems, and display successive attempts to solve them by self-repair processes. This chapter focuses on the problem sources, repair strategies, delay markers, and time PLI children take in giving a repair solution at a lexical level compared to Typical Language Development (TLD) control pairs. PLI and TLD children’s repair solution time (RST) may be affected by the complexity of problem-sources being repaired (nominal, verbal or functional targets), repair strategy (lexical searches, replacements or lexical reformulations), and number and types of delay markers used. Analysis was realized on 12 h of conversations of six monolingual, Spanish-speaking Mexican children (ages 6;2 to 6;6, three PLI and three age-matched TLD children). As expected, lexical searches were longer in children with PLI (Mdn = 1910 ms, IQR = 970) than in TLD children (Mdn = 1195 ms, IQR = 3020). In addition, PLI group took longer to achieve a lexical target through elaboration (Mdn = 540 ms, IQR = 670) than children with TLD (Mdn = 380, IQR = 910). These results suggest that both groups do monitoring activities of the same focal aspects of language, but PLI children confine themselves to a more restricted dimension as syllable sequences. About lexical searches, PLI children do not conduct latent searches and abruptly interrupt their discourse to insert the item they had previously been unable to find. TLD group relied more on overt markers (isolated or joined), in contrast to the more frequent pauses and immediate elaboration repairs in PLI children. As for the differences in RST, although we did not find statistically significant differences between the groups, (PLI: Mdn = 453 ms, IQR = 717; TLD: Mdn = 400 ms, IQR = 957.5); (U = 1410.50, p = 0.779), PLI children took longer time intervals in preparing a repair solution in every grammatical category. This data could be related to the evidence of unsettled lexical-semantic representations in PLI scholars.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Arias, J., & Lléo, C. (2009). Comparing the representation of iambs by monolingual German, monolingual Spanish and bilingual German-Spanish children. In K. Braunmüller & J. House (Eds.), Convergence and divergence in language contact situations (pp. 205–234). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Beverly, B. L., & Estis, J. M. (2003). Fast mapping deficits during disambiguation in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 407–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. (1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied PsychoLinguistics, 15, 507–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. (2004). Specific language impairment: Diagnostic dilemmas. In L. Verhoeven & V. Balkom (Eds.), Classification of developmental language disorders (pp. 309–326). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmer, E. R., & Mitton, J. L. (1991). Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech. Cognition, 39, 173–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, L., Oostdijk, N., & Boves, L. (2005). On temporal aspects of turn taking in conversational dialogues. Speech Communication, 47, 80–86. doi:10.1016/j.specom.2005.05.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casillas, M. (2014). Talking the floor on time: Delay and deferral in children’s turn-taking. In I. Arnon, M. Casillas, C. Kurumada, & B. Estigarribia (Eds.), Language in interaction: Studies in honour of Eve V. Clark (pp. 101–114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time. The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estis, J. M., & Beverly, B. L. (2015). Children with PLI exhibit delays resolving ambiguous reference. Journal of Child Language, 42(1), 180–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finneran, D. A., Francis, A., & Leonard, L. B. (2009). Sustained attention in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 915–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. (2006). Early N400 development and later language acquisition. Psychophysiology, 43, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C. (1984). Children’s talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(5–6), 613–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heldner, M., & Edlund, J. (2010). Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations. Journal of Phonetics, 38, 555–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson-Maldonado, D., Peña, E., & Aghara, R. (2011). Funciones de lenguaje y tipos de palabras en la interacción entre madres y sus hijos e hijas. In C. Rojas-Nieto & D. Jackson-Maldonado (Eds.), Interacción y uso lingüístico (pp. 27–62). México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendrick, K. H. (2015). The intersection of turn-taking and repair: The timing of the other-initiations of repair in conversation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 509. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitzinger, C. (2013). Repairs. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversational analiysis (pp. 229–258). Oxford: Wiley-Blackewell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kormos, J. (1999). The timing of self-repairs in second language speech production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 145–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornilov, S., Magnuson, J., Rakhlin, N., Landi, N., & Grigorenko, E. (2015). Lexical processing deficits in children with developmental language disorder: An event-related potentials study. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 459–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lara, W., Rojas-Nieto, C., Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Díaz, C. (2015). ¿Qué problemas fuente autorreparan los niños con Trastorno Primario de Lenguaje? El él, el súsape, el pícipe. Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada, 61, 165–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behaviour with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 41(3), 841–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B., Weismer, S., Miller, C., Francis, D., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication – Origins and implications for language processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 6–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010

  • Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F. (2015). Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 731. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48, 1138–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. (1993). Issues of process and representation in lexical access. In G. Altmann & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Cognitive models of language processes: Second Sperlonga Meeting. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60(1), 1–39. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.06.003

  • Melgar, M. (2002). Cómo detectar al niño con problemas del habla. México: Trillas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merryman, W. E., & Schuster, J. M. (1991). Young children’s disambiguation of object name reference. Child Development, 62, 1288–1301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? Nueva York: Dial Books for Young Readers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W. (2000). Relation of working memory to off-line and real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 21, 117–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W. (2002). Examining the nature of lexical processing in children with specific language impairment: Temporal processing or processing capacity deficit? Applied PsychoLinguistics, 23, 447–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). WM and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 78–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F., Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K., & Mather, N. (2005). Batería III Woodcock-Muñoz. Itacca: Riversi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1398–1411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). The effects of processing and sequence organization on the timing of turn taking: A corpus study. Frontiers in Psichology, 6, 509. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rojas-Nieto, C. (1998). La construcción del dato en el estudio de la adquisición del lenguaje. In S. Reyes Coria (Ed.), Jornadas Filológicas 1997: Memoria (pp. 23–30). México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabisch, B., Hahne, A., Galss, E., von Suchodoletz, W., & Friederici, A. (2006). Lexical-semantic processes in children with specific language impairment. Neuroreport, 17(14), 1511–1514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2000). Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking of conversation. Language in Society, 29, 1–63. doi:10.1017/S0047404500001019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. (2013). Ten operations in self-initiated, same-turn repair. In M. Hayashi, G. Raymond, & J. Sidnell (Eds.), Conversational repair and human understanding (pp. 41–70). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53(2), 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wayne, W., & Secord, A. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, Spanish edition (CELF-4). Psychological Corporation. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 146–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1977). The development conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1988). The children with specific language impairment. In R. J. McCauley (Ed.), Language, speech and reading disorders in children: Neuropsychological studies (pp. 56–114). San Diego: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., & Levinson, S. C. (2007). Person reference in interaction. In N. J. Enfield & T. Stivers (Eds.), Person reference in interaction: Linguistic, cultural and social perspectives (pp. 1–20). Cambride: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J., Yoon, K., & Levinson, S. (2009). Universal and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(26), 10587–10592. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903616106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Stark, R. (1981). Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normal developing and language-impaired children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 568–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2010). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velez, M., & Schwartz, R. G. (2010). Spoken word recognition in school-age children with SLI: Semantic, phonological, and repetition priming. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(6), 1616–1628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M., & Croft, W. (2007). Phonological development: Toward a “radical” templatic phonology. Linguistics, 45(4), 683–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para preescolar y Primaria (WIPPSI). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wendy Fabiola Lara Galindo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lara Galindo, W.F., Rojas-Nieto, C. (2017). Self-Repair Timing of Lexical Problem Sources: A Window into Primary Language Impairment Online Processing. In: Auza Benavides, A., Schwartz, R. (eds) Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children. Literacy Studies, vol 14. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_12

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-53645-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-53646-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics