1 Introduction

1.1 Acceleration

Alpine ski racers ski around poles which are used to describe the race line. Each ski turn creates a situation where there is a negative acceleration of the skier’s center-of-mass (CoM) followed by a period of positive acceleration between turns [1]. To ski fast in slalom, the ski racer skis with their skis close to the pole such that their CoM may actually travel to the inside of the race line. This is a legal passage according to the rules of the Fédération Internationale de Ski (FIS), the governing body of international ski racing. Racers will contact the long pole hitting, blocking, moving, or clearing it out of the way with their hand and or ski pole, allowing their body to take a shorter path. This pole contact and movement requires about 181 J for each pole clear for a World Cup ski racer which may explain part of their CoM-negative acceleration (R. Reid, personal communication, 2010). This negative acceleration would be amplified for young skiers having less mass.

While ski racers must ski around poles, recreational skiers make turns in voluntary locations and tempo. For recreational skiers, turning is an opportunity to manage speed while for the ski racer it is a situation for speed loss to be minimized.

1.2 Poles and Training Aids

Poles are used in alpine ski racing to define the race line for the ski racer. The FIS [2] has specifications for pole dimensions used in international competition, while individual countries determine domestic standards.

Poles have a hinge at the snow level. This hinge was developed to keep the pole from becoming dislodged as ski racers skied increasingly closer to the pole eventually discovering they could just mow down the pole with their hand, ski pole, or shin enabling them to take a shorter and therefore faster path. Standards for the hinge are mandated by the FIS.

The FIS has two categories of poles: Type A; which is for use in any FIS race. They have a shaft diameters from 29 to 32 mm. Type B poles (25–28.9 mm) are for use in any FIS race except World Cup.

Poles vary by their height above the snow surface. All FIS races must employ poles that project a minimum of 1.8 m above the snow surface [3]. In the United States, U16 competitions must also use the 1.8 m poles (FIS A or B). However, slalom poles for U14s must be 152 cm FIS B, while stubby poles are for non-scored events [4].

Young ski racers typically compete with shorter length (137 and 152 cm) and smaller diameter poles (27 vs. 32 mm). In place of poles, props or training aids, such as Whiskers and Heroes, reduce contact while still skiing the intended path. Whiskers are 23 cm brushes and Heroes are 70 cm flexible poles. There are no FIS standards for Whiskers and Heroes (SPM S.P.A., Varese, Italy).

The purpose of this study was to determine if pole type or shaft length had an effect on acceleration. Additionally, would finishing time, and subjective feelings of young ski racers be altered by pole length?

2 Methods

2.1 Subjects

Eighty seven (54 females, 33 males) club level skiers from three different USSA (United States Ski and Snowboard Association) affiliate ski teams participated in the study. Data are presented as average (± standard deviation). Average age was 12 years 10 days (±1.0 year). Skiers reported an average 4.6 (±0.9) years of race experience. Average height was 153.4 (±8.9) cm, average weight 41.05 (±10.88) kg, and average ski length was 140.4 (±7.80) cm. “Skilled” and “unskilled skiers” were defined by the nine skiers with the fastest and slowest run time. This was then confirmed with video by a USSA-certified ski coach.

2.2 Courses

The study was conducted on 12 different slopes at Bridger Bowl Ski Area, Montana, and Park City Mountain Resort, Utah, over 11 separate days in winter conditions. The 12 slopes averaged 20.9 degrees (±2.7). They were relatively even in pitch with minimal side hill. Individual average pitches consisted of 20, 23.5, 28, 22, 22, 18, 18, 20, 20, 19.4, 20, 20°.

An identical ten gate slalom course was set for all trials. It consisted of four open gates set at 9 m (gate to gate) to a three gate flush at 5.5 m followed by three open gates set at 9 m (gate to gate) to the finish 9 m away (see Fig. 1). Offset was 4 m with the flush in line with the same 4 m horizontal distance. All courses were slipped frequently in order to minimize rut development and overall course deterioration.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Course set-up used for all data collection

Skiers completed one or more runs on each pole type course in a random order. Multiple runs on the same course were used to check for reliability between runs.

2.2.1 Pole Type Used During Each Testing

  • Whiskers: Bright orange and green plastic were alternated in the courses. 23 cm length, with a lightly threaded 20.5 cm base.

  • Hero: Red and blue, pliable, rubber-like shaft, 35 mm in diameter and 70 cm long. 86 cm overall length including BrushGrip™ Base.

  • 137 cm: 27 mm diameter shaft that is 137 cm off the snow with BrushGrip™ Base.

  • 152 cm: 27 mm diameter shaft that is 152 cm off the snow with BrushGrip™ Base.

  • 180 cm: 27 mm diameter shaft that is 180 cm off the snow with BrushGrip™ Base.

All poles, Heroes, and Whiskers were manufactured by SPM and were new at the beginning of the study. Pole hinges showed no sign of deterioration at the end of the study.

2.2.2 Timing

The order in which the courses were skied was randomized for each testing bout.

Timing was with a Brower XS Timing System (Salt Lake City, UT, USA).

2.2.3 Acceleration

A gyroscopically controlled three-dimensional accelerometer (Electronic Realization, Bozeman, MT) was attached to the crown of the skier’s helmet. Sagittal plane, the fore/aft direction was defined as “x” the vertical direction as “y” the lateral direction as “z”.

2.2.4 Skier Perception

Skier perception immediately at the conclusion of each run was collected. The subject responded to four statements indicating their perception about speed, aggressiveness, confidence, and line with a 10-point Likert scale.

On that course I felt:

  • Slow…fast

  • Not aggressive…very aggressive

  • Not confident…very confident

  • Not a good line…a very good line

The higher the response number, the more positive the skier felt. A single factor ANOVA and post hoc Tukey HSD were used to calculate significance.

3 Results

3.1 Time

For the analysis 522 valid runs were used.

Average run time and standard deviation (SD) in seconds per pole type (Table 1):

Table 1 Average run time ± SD (s) per pole or training aid type

Statistically (single factor ANOVA and post hoc Tukey HSD):

  • Whiskers and Heroes times are not different from each other.

  • Whiskers and Hero times are different from 137 cm, 152 cm, 180 cm (p = 0.01).

  • 137 cm and 152 cm times are not different from each other.

  • 137 cm and 152 cm times are different from Whiskers, Heroes (p = 0.01), 180 cm (p = 0.01, p = 0.05).

  • 180 cm times are different from all other poles (p = 0.01), 137 cm (p = 0.05).

3.1.1 Reliability Between Runs of Same Pole Type (Within Skier)

Determining the variability between run times was important to establish test-retest reliability. 141 duplicate runs from the five pole/training aids were used to determine reliability. One factor to be noted was that the runs used were all from the same measurement day. Due to the multivariate nature of ski racing, this was the best option to control for the many variables that would most likely change from day to day.

A very high reliability [Pearson Product-Moment Correlation coefficient (r)] was found for all pole and training aid types (Table 2):

Table 2 Reliability for each pole and training aid type

3.2 Perception

Skiers showed consistent trends in perception after their conclusion.

3.2.1 Questionnaire Results (Table 3 )

Table 3 Results from the 0–10-point Likert scales asked at the conclusion of each run

3.3 Acceleration

3.3.1 Mean Acceleration Skilled vs. Less Skilled

There were significant differences for mean sagittal plane helmet acceleration between the three pole types and the two training aids. Also, a significant difference was observed when the skiers were grouped into “skilled” and “less skilled” cohorts (p = 0.0000). The skilled skiers having a positive acceleration while the less skilled a negative acceleration. See Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Helmet acceleration across pole type

There was also a significant difference in mean acceleration between the skilled and less skilled in the vertical (p = 0.000) and lateral (p = 0.000) directions.

There were no significant differences in mean acceleration in any of the three directions between the five pole types, nor was there any difference when grouped into the two training aids (Whisker and Hero) and the two longer pole lengths (152 and 180 cm) for any of the three directions. See Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean accelerations in the fore/aft (x), vertical (y), and lateral (z) direction for the three pole lengths and two training aids for skilled and less skilled ski racers

3.4 Peak Acceleration

When averaged over ten consecutive turns there was significant differences for peak acceleration for the skilled group in the z direction (p = 0.03) when the short props were compared to the two long poles, and for the less skilled in the y direction (p = 0.05). When the short props were compared to the two long poles, all other short props vs. tall poles were nonsignificant.

4 Discussion

Three pole types (lengths) and two training aids were selected for this study. The 27 mm shaft diameter was chosen over the 25 mm shaft based upon a previous USSA club questionnaire and US supplier recommendations. The 137 and 152 cm lengths were chosen as they are popular lengths used in the United States for young ski racers, and 180 cm as the FIS standard. Whiskers and Heroes were included as they are popular training choices.

The subjects were very homogenous in age. The control of age was important to eliminate or reduce age as an extraneous variable that could potentially affect the results. The study was conducted over 11 nonconsecutive days on 12 differing slopes. This helped to ensure that the results would be representative to the population in question.

Acceleration was positive for the skilled skiers and negative for the less skilled skiers across all three pole lengths and the two training aids. The question must be asked, was this difference in acceleration due to the pole length or type, or was it indicative of the skilled behavior?

Did pole type effect time? Whisker and Heroes were the fastest, with the 137 and 152 cm next, followed by 180 cm as the slowest. This time transition from shorter pole to longer pole agrees with intuitive logic and could be based on the mechanical energy dissipation consequence of shaft deformation, shaft acceleration, and hinge resistance, or possibly the influence of pole size on the racer’s psychological state.

The run time of Whiskers and Heroes were not statistically different from one another. Contact with the pole or any line alteration with the Hero did not seem to influence the finish time when compared to the Whiskers. This was confirmed with the skier’s subjective impression giving a similar response (Whisker: 8.04, Hero: 7.85) when asked if they felt “slow” (0) or “very fast” (10). The skiers also report feeling less aggressive on the Hero (7.97) courses than the Whisker (8.25) courses (p = 0.01).

The 137 and 152 cm poles were not significantly different from each other in time. However, they were significantly different from the Whisker, Hero, and 180 cm. While the skier’s time was not statistically different, they did report feeling less confident and less satisfied with their line when skiing the 152 cm pole.

The 180 cm poles elicited the slowest time, which was also the feeling generated from the skiers. The skiers also felt less aggressive and less confident along with less satisfaction with their line.

A very high reliability was found for all five pole types (cumulatively r = 0.967). Reliability is an essential component of validity which was essential for this study. Being assured that a run time per pole type was indeed indicative of that pole type was crucial to the interpretation of the results.

4.1 Possible Explanations

There are natural acceleration consequences of a skier center-of-mass during a ski turn. Reid and coworkers have used kinematic data with elite ski racers showing that during a slalom turn, the skier’s CoM goes through a period of negative acceleration that is followed by a period of positive acceleration between turns [1]. The period of negative acceleration starts when the turn is initiated which is before flex pole contact. Recently, Schindelwig and coworkers used finite element analysis to demonstrate the increase in race time due to a longer flex pole in children’s slalom racing [5].

4.1.1 Physical Contact with the Pole

It has been calculated that elite ski racers contacting an FIS A pole will use 45 J for work to deform the pole, 108 J to accelerate the pole, and 28 J to overcome hinge resistance for 181 J of work. This comes out to be about 4% of his kinetic energy (R. Reid, personal communication, 2010). Upon video analysis, the less skilled skiers did not necessarily contact the pole.

4.1.2 Anticipatory Postural Control

Racers contact the pole by hitting, blocking, moving, or clearing it out of the way with their hand or ski pole allowing their body to take a shorter path. The action to perform this “clearing” maneuver involves raising the arm which is accomplished with the pectoralis major and anterior fibers of the deltoid. While this is the mechanism, a phenomena observed and described by Belen’kii et al. [6] that standing subjects when asked to raise their arm, utilized both their prime movers (arm) and their postural muscles (trunk and legs). The postural muscles innervate 50 ms prior to the prime mover activation. This was in anticipation the upcoming destabilizing effects of the arm movement.

The more skilled ski racers could be anticipating the impact of pole contact with their postural muscles. These could be with the lower body or the neck muscles. In such a case, they are accelerating their upper body in proactive manner intuitively knowing that the pole will have a negative accelerating effect on them. This anticipatory postural adjustment has been shown to be a learned response by Cordo and Nashner [7]. The less skilled ski racers may not have had ample experience to have sufficiently learned this anticipatory postural mechanism and partially explain the resultant negative acceleration seen in this study.

4.1.3 Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex

The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) maintains the visual image during linear and angular head movement. While skiing skill may be thought of as the ability to manipulate the skis in an efficient strategy, there may be an adaptive mechanisms in the central nervous system that is important in maintaining the VOR [8, 9]. This adaptation may be a result of increasing skiing skill or possibly a manifestation of maturity.

VOR motor learning functions to improve motor performance. This occurs via the gain of the VOR or the ratio of eye to head speed and its phase which is the temporal relationship between eye and head movement. Eye movements are opposite in direction and approximately equal in speed to the head movement [10]. It could be postulated that the positive acceleration from the skilled skiers at pole contact was an anticipatory learned response knowing that the pole would provide some negative acceleration at contact.

4.1.4 Psychological Factors

One explanation for the positive and negative acceleration at pole contact from the skilled and less skilled can be explained by the novelty of situation. The skilled skier has encountered this situation many times and knows what to expect. The less skilled skier is not as sure what to expect. The less skilled skiers’ negative helmet acceleration may be a result of moving away from the pole in a ducking or avoidance response.

4.1.5 Ski Pressure Management

Elite ski racers strive to pressure their skis while they are in the fall-line or in the part of the turn that is pointed down the hill. Less skilled ski racers and recreational skiers that are striving for speed control as opposed to speed conservation will have pressure later in the turn. This type of turn will not accelerate in the turn but constantly show a negative acceleration throughout the turn.

4.1.6 Movement Decision Under Risk

Alpine skiing can be perceived as a risky endeavor. The skier skill level can lead to behavior that is based on risk avoidance or risk-seeking objectives [11, 12]. A risk avoidance situation can be postulated from the less skilled ski racers that will have greater variability in most aspects of their skiing technique suggesting increased instability [13]. Some of the negative acceleration seen in this study could be explicated by the risk perception that occurs during the middle of the turn when the skis are pointed at their most down the hill attitude and coinciding with a possible pole contact. Both could be assumed to be a “risky” moment in time.

4.2 What Does It Mean for Safety?

There are many ways to become injured skiing. The most obvious is losing some level of control of the skis. When injury does happen, there is a 50% chance it is to the lower extremity, which today usually means an ACL injury [14, 15]. Most of the ACL injuries among recreational skiers are reported to occur during falling [16, 17]. The three mechanisms of ACL injury all involve falling either in a backward or a forward direction. [18]. Specifically, the “phantom-foot,” “boot-induced anterior drawer,” “slip-catch,” “landing back-weighted,” and the “dynamic snowplow” mechanisms all have the skier falling backwards while the “valgus-external rotation” the skier falls forward. This is different from the mechanisms for other sports, such as American football, basketball, and soccer. In these activities, the foot is in firm contact with the ground and usually results in deceleration, change-of-direction, or some sort of direct blow. In skiing, the skis are constantly sliding forward while not in contact with the ground. This forward motion constantly challenges the skier to maintain control of their fore/aft relationship.

Fore/aft balance has always been a nemesis to the skier. The importance to recreational skiers is that the Professional Ski Instructor’s of American lists “controlling the relationship of the center-of-mass to the base of support to direct pressure along the length of the skis” as one of their five Fundamentals of Skiing [19]. And among elite ski racers, fore/aft movement has been found to be correlated with speed in a race course [20, 21].

While it would be inappropriate to suggest that having superior fore/aft balance would lead to greater control, and therefore greater safety, it would conversely be irresponsible to overlook the relevancy and connection between sagittal plane control and the possible association with safety.