Skip to main content

Thinking, Values and Meaning in Changing Cognitive Ecologies

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognition Beyond the Brain

Abstract

In defence of the pluralism of Cognition Beyond the Brain, we propose a systemic view of cognition. We argue that this opens up the diversity of thinking by allowing persons to act as living subjects who also make routine use of collectively organised cognitive ecosystems (Hutchins 2014). To allow for the duality of human agency , one needs a methodological principle that links subjective concerns with collective control . The principle of cognitive separability (PCS) posits that people can dissociate extended causal processes from subject-centred actions . The systemic view offers three main claims about human cognition . First, language , problem solving , human-computer interaction , and many elaborate cognitive skills are based in sense-saturated coordination or interactivity . Second, interactivity prompts embrained bodies to develop the cognitive powers used in human performance . Third, human agents entwine brains, bodies and world as they use the PCS to manage multi-scalar dynamics. In this second edition of Cognition beyond the brain, we defend the principles by presenting the state of the art. The papers address, among other things, use of the internet, the fluid nature of self, the history of the Paris Commune , and computer aided design . In line with cognitive separability, the studies vary in the granularity used to pursue how bodies and artifacts shape thinking . Whereas some contributors invoke (putative) mental content , others take the radical view that thinking depends on non-localisable pattern . Yet, in all cases, experience of materiality is meshed with activity that involves the world beyond the body.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    ‘Relevance’ tends to be used when mainstream values and discourse are used to justify the parochial concerns of individuals, organizations and political bodies.

  2. 2.

    “Nevertheless, there remains a fundamental asymmetry in my view. I want to call a system cognitive because it produces cognitive outputs, but refuse to call it knowledgeable because it produces knowledge. For me, the latter makes as little sense as calling a system edible because it was designed to produce edibles” (Giere 2011: 397).

  3. 3.

    Douglas Adams (1979).

  4. 4.

    The hypothesis of extended mind Clark and Chalmers (1998) posits a parity principle that allows resources beyond the brain to be partly constitutive of cognition (qua function). For Sutton (2010), the question is better addressed in relation to how the external complements the psychological; for Rupert (2010), cognition is a ‘phenomenon’ whereby a person adverts to “a persisting, integrated set of mechanisms, capacities, abilities, etc.” There are many other views: indeed, even the dual control of the PCS has a bearing on these debates.

  5. 5.

    While this artificial kind of interactivity pertains to an individual’s thinking—not processes between people—it shows how a machine can sponsor human actions.

References

  • Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, D. (1979). The hitchhiker’s guide to the galaxy. London: Pan Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aston, A. (this vol.). Unruly minds in the city of light: Cognitive ecology and the Paris Commune of 1871. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baber, C. (this vol.). Distributed cognition at the crime scene. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, L. J., & Lichfield, D. (this vol.). Interactivity and embodied cues in problem solving and insight: Further contributions to a “theory of hints”. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben Naim, J., Bonnefon, J. F., Herzig, A., Leblois, S., & Lorini, E. (this vol.). Computer-mediated trust in self-interested expert recommendation. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz, A. (2012). On simplexity: Simplifying principles for a complex world. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bottineau, D. (2012). Remembering voice past: Languaging as an embodied interactive cognitive technique. In: Proceedings of the Humanities International Forum, Russian State University for the Humanities, Moscow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2013). Radical embodied cognitive science. Review of General Psychology, 17, 145–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. (2011). Distributed language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. (2014). Linguistic embodiment and verbal constraints: Human cognition and the scales of time. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1085. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2010). Thinking in action. AI & SOCIETY, 25, 469–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2013). Systemic cognition: Human artifice in life and language. In S. J. Cowley, and F. Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (Eds.), cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice, pp. 255–273. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1998). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (2011). Incomplete nature: How mind emerged from matter. WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fioratou, E., & Cowley, S. J. (2009). Insightful thinking: Cognitive dynamics and material artifacts. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 549–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2004). The problem of agency in scientific distributed cognitive systems. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 759–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2011). Distributed cognition as human centered although not human bound: Reply to vaesen. Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy, 25, 393–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, M. I., Gahrn-Andersen, R., & Steffensen, S. V. (2016). Interactivity and enaction in human cognition. Constructivist Foundations, 11(2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, B. H. (2007). Good prospects: Ecological and social perspectives on conforming, creating, and caring in conversation. Language Sciences, 29(5), 584–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, J., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000). Distributed cognition: Toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 7, 174–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 34–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2012). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaynes, J. (2000). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Järvilehto, T. (1998). The theory of the organism-environment system: I. Description of the theory. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 33, 321–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Järvilehto, T. (2009). The theory of the organism-environment system as a basis of experimental work in psychology. Ecological Psychology, 21, 112–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, P. E. (2013). You want a piece of me? Paying your dues and getting your due in a distributed world. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (pp. 113–129). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kiousis, S. (2002). Interactivity: A concept explication. New Media and Society, 4, 355–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (1997). Interactivity and multimedia interfaces. Instructional Science, 25(2), 79–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (this vol.). Thinking with external representations. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravchenko, A. (2007). Essential properties of language: Why language is not a digital code. Language Sciences, 29, 650–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravchenko, A. V. (2009). The experiential basis of speech and writing as different cognitive domains. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17(3), 527–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Love, N. (2004). Cognition and the language myth. Language Sciences, 26, 525–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, J. K. (this vol.). Time during time: Multi-scalar temporal cognition. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind: The Journal of Archaeology, Consciousness and Culture, 8, 351–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markoš, A. Švorcová, J. & Lhotský, (this vol.). Within the skin—and beyond. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, C. W. (1938). Foundations of the theory of signs. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2009). Out of our heads: Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness. Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge MA: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, M. and Cowley, S. J. (this vol.). Agency, languaging and the resources of reason. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd Edition). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, S. B. (2012). Interactivity in health care: Bodies, values and dynamics. Language Sciences, 34(5), 532–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, S. B. (2015). The cognitive ecology of human errors in emergency medicine: An interactivity-based approach. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Southern Denmark.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, M. (this vol). Socially distributed cognition in loosely coupled systems. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd Edition). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulsgaard, K. S., & Malafouris, L. (this vol.). Models, mathematics and materials in digital architecture: Creative imagination at the human-computer interface. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Port, R. F. (2010). Rich memory and distributed phonology. Language Sciences, 32(1), 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. (2007). H. sapiens as ecologically special: What does language contribute? Language Sciences, 29/5: 710–731. R. D. Rupert. (2009). Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rupert, R. D. (2010). Extended cognition and the priority of cognitive systems. Cognitive Systems Research, 11(4), 343–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Secchi, D., & Cowley, S. (2016). Organisational cognition. In Euram Conference Proceedings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Secchi, D., & Adamsen, B. (this vol.). Organisational cognition: A step too close. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smart, P., Heersmink, R., & Clowes, R. (this vol.). The cognitive ecology of the web. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steffensen, S. V. (this vol.). Human interactivity: Problem-finding, problem-solving, and verbal patterns in the wild. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind (pp. 189–225). Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vallée-Tourangeau, F., & Villejoubert, G. (2013). Naturalising problem solving. In cognition beyond the brain (pp. 241–253). London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallée-Tourangeau, G. & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (this vol.). Cognition beyond the classical information processing model: Cognitive interactivity and the systemic thinking model (SysTM). In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Interactivity, computation and human artifice (2nd ed.). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott, D. W. (1969). The use of an object. The International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 50, 711–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. W. (1958). Philosophical investigations (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen J. Cowley .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cowley, S.J., Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2017). Thinking, Values and Meaning in Changing Cognitive Ecologies. In: Cowley, S., Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (eds) Cognition Beyond the Brain. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49115-8_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49115-8_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-49114-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-49115-8

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics