Advertisement

Should Investigators Introspect on Their Own Pain Experiences as Study Co-Participants?

  • Simon van RysewykEmail author
  • Carl L. von Baeyer
Chapter

Abstract

The question of investigators introspecting on their own personal pain experiences in pain studies has received little attention in the literature. Study of this question may reflect ethical reservations about the many points at which self-interest may lead us to introspect on personal experiences through personal biases that in turn impair professional decision-making and perception. Despite this valid concern about research co-participation, we offer three reasons why investigators can introspect on personal pain as co-participants in their own pain studies. First, there is historical precedent for investigator participation and co-participation in scientific pain research using introspection as a study method. Second, general concerns about variability in self-report based on introspection on pain experience partly derive from true fluctuations in personal pain experience and perceived interests in self-reporting pain, not simply error in its scientific measurement. Third, the availability of the experiential–phenomenological method, a mixed research method for the study of human experiences, allows investigators to co-participate with naïve participants in their own studies by encouraging passive introspection on personal pain experiences.

Keywords

Pain Experience Pain Catastrophizing Mixed Research Method Pain Research Phenomenological Method 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Apkarian AV, Hashmi JA, Baliki MN (2011) Pain and the brain: specificity and plasticity of the brain in clinical chronic pain. Pain 152:S49–S64CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrell JJ, Barrell JE (1975) A self-directed approach for a science of human experience. J Phenomenol Psychol 6(1):63–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boring EG (1953) A history of introspection. Psychol Bull 50(3):169–189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bushnell CM, Taylor MB, Duncan GH, Dubner R (1983) Discrimination of innocuous and noxious thermal stimuli applied to the face in human and monkey. Somatosens Mot Res 1(2):119–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chalmers DJ (1996) The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  6. Coghill RC, McHaffie JG, Yen YF (2003) Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:8538–8542CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Craig KD (2006) Emergent pain language communication competence in infants and children. Enfance 58(1):52–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Craig KD (2009) The social communication model of pain. Can Psychol 50(1):22–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davis KD, Racine E, Collett B (2012) Neuroethical issues related to the use of brain imaging: can we and should we use brain imaging as a biomarker to diagnose chronic pain? Pain 153(8):1555–1559CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. deCharms RC, Maeda F, Glover GH, Ludlow D, Pauly JM, Soneji D, Gabreli JDE, Mackey SC (2005) Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102(51):18626–18631Google Scholar
  11. Dresser R (2013) Personal knowledge and study participation. J Med Ethics. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2013-101390
  12. Elander J, Robinson G, Mitchell K, Morris J (2009) An assessment of the relative influence of pain coping, negative thoughts about pain, and pain acceptance on health-related quality of life among people with hemophilia. Pain 145(1):169–175CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Flor H, Fydrich T, Turk DC (1992) Efficacy of multidisciplinary pain treatment centers: a meta-analytic review. Pain 49:221–230CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Gallagher S, Sørensen JB (2006) Experimenting with phenomenology. Conscious Cog15(1):119–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hadjistavropoulos T, Craig KD, Duck S, Cano A, Goubert L, Jackson PL, Mogil JS, Rainville P, Sullivan MJL, de C Williams A, Vervoort T, Fitzgerald TD (2011) A biopsychosocial formulation of pain communication. Psychol Bull 137(6):910–939CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Hardy JD, Wolff HG, Goodell H (1940) Studies on pain: a new method for measuring pain threshold: observations on spatial summation of pain. J Clin Investig 19(4):649–657Google Scholar
  17. Head H (1920) Studies in neurology. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoffman BM, Papas RK, Chatkoff DK, Kerns RD (2007) Meta-analysis of psychological interventions for chronic low back pain. Health Psychol 26:1–9CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM (2001) Changes in beliefs, catastrophizing, and coping are associated with improvement in multidisciplinary pain treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 69(4):655CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Landau W, Bishop GH (1953) Pain from dermal, periosteal, and fascial endings and from inflammation: electrophysiological study employing differential nerve blocks. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 69(4):490–504CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Letzen JE, Boissoneault J, Sevel LS, Robinson ME (2016) Test–retest reliability of pain-related functional brain connectivity compared with pain self-report. Pain 157(3):546–551CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. McCrystal KN, Craig KD, Versloot J, Fashler SR, Jones DN (2011) Perceiving pain in others: validation of a dual processing model. Pain 152(5):1083–1089CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Melzack R (1984) Neuropsychological basis of pain measurement. Adv Pain Res Ther 6:323–339Google Scholar
  24. Mountcastle VB (1974) Pain and temperature sensibilities. Med Physiol 13(1):348–391Google Scholar
  25. O’Leart H, Smart KM, Moloney NA, Doody CM (2016) Nervous system sensitization as a predictor of outcome in the treatment of peripheral musculoskeletal conditions: a systematic review. Pain Pract doi: 10.1111/papr.12484 Google Scholar
  26. Perls F, Hefferline RF, Goodman P (1951) Gestalt Therapy. Dell, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  27. Price DD (1972) Characteristics of second pain and flexion reflexes indicative of prolonged central summation. Exp Neurol 37(2):371–387CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Price DD, Barrell J (1980) An experiential approach with quantitative methods—a research paradigm. J Hum Psych 20(3):75–95Google Scholar
  29. Price DD (2000) Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288(5472):1769–1772Google Scholar
  30. Price DD, Aydede M (2006) The experimental use of introspection in the scientific study of pain and its integration with third-person methodologies: the experiential–phenomenological approach. In: Aydede M (ed) Pain: new essays on its nature and the methodology of its study. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 243–273Google Scholar
  31. Price DD, Barrell JJ (2012) Inner experiences and neuroscience. Merging the two perspectives. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  32. Price DD, Hu JW, Dubner R, Gracely RH (1977) Peripheral suppression of first pain and central summation of second pain evoked by noxious heat pulses. Pain 3(1):57–68CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Price DD, Barrell JJ, Rainville P (2002) Integrating experiential–phenomenological methods and neuroscience to study neural mechanisms of pain and consciousness. Conscious Cog 11(4):593–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Robinson ME, Staud R, Price DD (2013) Pain measurement and brain activity: will neuroimages replace pain ratings? J Pain 14(4):323–327CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Rosier EM, Iadarola MJ, Coghill RC (2002) Reproducibility of pain measurement and pain perception. Pain 98:205–216CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Salomons TV, Moayedi M, Erpelding N, Davis KD (2014) A brief cognitive-behavioural intervention for pain reduces secondary hyperalgesia. Pain 155(8):1446–1452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schiavenato M, Craig KD (2010) Pain assessment as a social transaction: beyond the “gold standard”. Clin J Pain 26(8):667–676PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Stanford EA, Chambers CT, Craig KD (2005) A normative analysis of the development of pain-related vocabulary in children. Pain 114(1):278–284CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Staud R, Vierck CJ, Cannon RL, Mauderli AP, Price DD (2001) Abnormal sensitization and temporal summation of second pain (wind-up) in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain 91(1):165–175CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Staud R, Robinson ME, Vierck CJ Jr, Cannon RC, Mauderli AP, Price DD (2003) Ratings of experimental pain and pain-related negative affect predict clinical pain in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain 105:215–222CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Staud R, Craggs JG, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME, Price DD (2008) Brain activity associated with slow temporal summation of C-fiber evoked pain in fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls. Eur J Pain 12(8):1078–1089CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Sternbach RA (1968) Pain: a psychophysiological analysis. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J (1995) The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 7(4):524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. van Rysewyk S (2014) Objective knowledge of subjective pain? Towards a subjective-neuroscience of pain. Ngau Mamae 10–20Google Scholar
  45. von Baeyer CL (2006) Children’s self-reports of pain intensity: scale selection, limitations and interpretation. Pain Res Manag 11:157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Woo CW, Wager TD (2016) What reliability can and cannot tell us about pain report and pain neuroimaging. Pain 157(3):511–513CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Woolf SH, Zimmerman E, Haley A, Krist AH (2016) Authentic engagement of patients and communities can transform research, practice, and policy. Health Aff 35(4):590–594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Woollard HH, Carmichael EA (1933) The testis and referred pain. Brain 56(3):293–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wortolowska K (2011) How neuroimaging can help us to visualise and quantify pain? Eur J Pain 5:323–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Humanities, Department of Philosophy and Gender StudiesUniversity of TasmaniaHobartAustralia
  2. 2.Department of Clinical Health PsychologyUniversity of ManitobaWinnipegCanada
  3. 3.Department of Pediatrics and Child HealthUniversity of ManitobaWinnipegCanada

Personalised recommendations