Advertisement

Another Biolinguistics History: From Aristotle to Darwin

  • Antonino Pennisi
  • Alessandra Falzone
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology book series (PEPRPHPS, volume 12)

Abstract

This chapter reconstructs a possible alternative history of Chomskyan biolinguistics hinging on the embodiment of language, whose precursors are identified in Aristotle, Vico, Darwin and Broca. The recent re-reading of Aristotle, given by Franco Lo Piparo, is used to reconstruct a biolinguistics independent of the cerebrocentric hypothesis and, therefore, entirely focused on the primacy of linguistic articulation and of the relevant cognitive properties. Vico reconstructs the glottogonic dimension as the first truly linguistic evolutionary epistemology. Darwin’s position on language is critically discussed in great detail in its functionalist aspects, often influenced by Lamarckian residues. However, we emphasize the decisive importance of gradualism of structures and the populational thought as elements of an irreversible biolinguistics turn materializing in the birth of neuroscience with the discoveries of Broca’s aphasia.

Keywords

Vocal Tract Human Language Nonhuman Animal Language Faculty Natural Speech 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Al-Mutairi, F. R. (2014). The minimalist program the nature and plausibility of Chomsky’s biolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allen, J. S. (2009). The lives of the brain: Human evolution and the organ of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bateman, F. (1877). Darwinism tested by language. London: Rivingtons.Google Scholar
  4. Bébian, P. A. (1817). Essai sur les sourds-mutes et sur le langage naturel, ou introduction a une classification naturele des idées avec leurs signes propres. Paris: J.G. Dentu.Google Scholar
  5. Bell, C. (1824a). Essay on the anatomy and philosophy of expression. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  6. Bell, C. (1824b). On the nerves of the face. London: Richard Taylor.Google Scholar
  7. Berti, E. (2004). Nuovi studi aristotelici. Brescia: Morcelliana.Google Scholar
  8. Bishop, J. (1851). On articulate sounds; and on the cause and cure of impediments of speech. London: Samuel Highley.Google Scholar
  9. Bishop, J. (1856). On the cause and treatment of impediments of speech; including the theory of articulate sounds, and the construction of hearing and speaking instruments. London: Joun Churchill.Google Scholar
  10. Black, J. M. (1988). Preflight signaling in swans. A mechanism for group cohesion and flock formation. Ethology, 79, 143–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boeckx, C. (2011). Biolinguistics: A brief guide for the perplexed. Language Science, 5, 449–463.Google Scholar
  12. Boeckx, C. (2013a). Biolinguistics: Fact, fiction, and forecast. Biolinguistics, 7, 316–328.Google Scholar
  13. Boeckx, C. (2013b). Biolinguistics: Forays into human cognitive biology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 91, 63–89.Google Scholar
  14. Boeckx, C., & Grohmann, K. K. (2007). The biolinguistics manifesto. Biolinguistics, 1, 1–8.Google Scholar
  15. Boeckx, C., & Grohmann, K. K. (2013). The Cambridge handbook of biolinguistics. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bouton, C. P. (1984). Discours physique du langage. Genès et histoire de la Neurolinguistique. Paris: PUF.Google Scholar
  17. Broca, P. P. (1888). IV. Morphologie comparée de l’homme et des primates. In Mémoires d’anthropologie. Paris: Reinwald, 465 ad finem.Google Scholar
  18. Busnel, R. G., & Fish, J. F. (1980). Animal sonar systems. In International interdisciplinary symposium on animal sonar systems. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  19. Cardella, V., Falzone, A., & Pennisi, A. (2013). From individual minds to social ones. Cooperation and the structure of animal and human societies. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Cooperative minds: Social interaction and group dynamics. Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 275–280). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  20. Cerise, L. (1865). Du systéme nerveux considéré dans ses rapports avec le langage et les institutions sociales. In L. Cerise (Ed.), Des fonctions et des maladies nerveuses dans leurs rapports avec l’éducation sociale et privée, morale et physique (pp.1–73). Paris: Masson (1870).Google Scholar
  21. Clarke, E., & Dewhurst, K. (1974). An illustrated history of brain function. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  22. Conradt, L., & Roper, T. J. (2003). Group decision-making in animals. Nature, 421, 156–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Conradt, L., & Roper, T. J. (2005). Consensus decision making in animals. Trends in ecology and evolution, 20(8), 449–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Croce, B. (1911). G.B. Vico. Bari: Laterza (1962).Google Scholar
  25. Cunnane, S. C. (2005). Survival of the fattest. The key to human brain evolution. Singapore: World Scientific Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Darwin, C. (1842) and (1844). The foundation of the origin of species. Two essays written in 1842 and 1844 by Charles Darwin. Cambridge Library Collection. Cambridge: Cambdrige University Press (2009).Google Scholar
  27. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origins of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray [D. G. Beer (ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2008].Google Scholar
  28. Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of man and selection in relation to sex (1st ed.). London: John Murray [Cambdrige University Press. 2009].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals (1st ed.). London: John Murray (Cambdrige University Press 2009).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Darwin, Ch. (1874). The Descent of man and selection in relation to sex. 2nd edition of 1874. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  31. Di Sciullo, A. M. (2009). The biolinguistics network. Biolinguistics, 2(4), 1–11.Google Scholar
  32. Di Sciullo, A. M., & Boeckx, C. (2011). The biolinguistic enterprise. New perspective on the evolution and nature on the human language faculty. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Falcon, A. (2005). Aristotle and the science of nature. Unity without uniformity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Freud, S. (1891). On aphasia; a critical study. New York: International Universities Press (1953).Google Scholar
  35. Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1999).Google Scholar
  36. Freud, S. (1904). The psychopathology of everyday life. London: Penguin (1973).Google Scholar
  37. Freud, S. (1905). Jokes and their relation to the unconscious. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul (1966).Google Scholar
  38. Gensini, S. (2014). Darwin’s view of language in the Descent of man. An intertextual reading. Human Evolution, 29(4), 303–318.Google Scholar
  39. Givón, T. (2002). Bio-linguistics. The Santa Barbara lectures. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gotthelf, A. (2012). Teleology, first principles, and scientific method in Aristotle’s biology. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gotthelf, A., & Lennox, J. G. (1987). Philosophical issues in Aristotle’s biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Gould, S. J. (2002). The structure of evolutionary theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Gross, C. G. (1998). Brain, vision, memory. Tales in the history of neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  44. Head, H. (1915). Hughlings Jackson on aphasia and kindred affection of speech. Brain. Part I-II, 38, 1–27.Google Scholar
  45. Héçaen, H., & Lantéri-Laura, G. (1977). Evolution des connaissances et des doctrines sur la localisation cérébrales. Paris: Desclee De Brouwer.Google Scholar
  46. Helmholtz, H. (1868). Théorie physiologique de la musique: Fondée sur l’étude des sensations auditives. Paris: Masson [On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music. Ellis, A. J. (ed.). New York: Dover Publications. 1954] [Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage für die Theorie der Musik, Unveränd. Nachdr. d. Ausg. Braunschweig 1863, Frankfurt/Main: Minerva-Verlag 1981].Google Scholar
  47. Helmholtz, H. (1869). In A. J. Ellis (Ed.), The mechanism of the ossicles of the ear and membrana tympani. New York: Dover Publications. 1873.Google Scholar
  48. Hublin, J. J., & Richards, M. P. (Eds.). (2009). The evolution of hominin diets: Integrating approaches to the study of palaeolithic subsistence. Dordrecht: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
  49. Jackson, H. (1866). Notes on the physiology and patology of language, in Jackson, 1931, 121 et seq.Google Scholar
  50. Jackson, H. (1868). On the physiology of language, in Jackson, 1931, 65–74Google Scholar
  51. Jackson, H. (1878–1879). On affection of speech from disease of the brain. In Jackson, 1931: 155 et seq.Google Scholar
  52. Jackson, H. (1879–1880). On affection of speech from disease of the brain. In Head, 1915, p1., 107 et seq.Google Scholar
  53. Jenkins, L. (2000). Biolinguistics exploring the biology of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Lamarck, J. B. (1809). Philosophie zoologique, ou exposition des considerations relatives a l’histoire naturelle des animaux, 2 vol. Paris: Dentu.Google Scholar
  55. Laspia, P. (1997). L’articolazione linguistica. Origini biologiche di una metafora. Roma: NIS.Google Scholar
  56. Lecours, A. R., & Lhermitte, F. (1979). L’aphasie. Paris: Flammarion.Google Scholar
  57. Leunissen, M. (2010). Explanation and teleology in Aristotle’s science of nature. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Lieberman, P. (1975). On the origins of language. An introduction to the evolution of human speech. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  59. Lieberman, P. (2012). Vocal tract anatomy and the neural bases of talking. Journal of Phonetics, 40(4), 608–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Lo Piparo, F. (2003). Aristotele e il linguaggio. Cosa fa di una lingua una lingua. Roma: Laterza.Google Scholar
  61. Lordat, J. (1843). Leçon tirées du cours de physiologie de l’année scolaire 1842–43. In H. Dubois (1969). 130–170Google Scholar
  62. Marie, P. (1907). Sur la fonction du langage. Revue de Philosophie, 1–23.Google Scholar
  63. Marie, P. (1922). Travaux et mémoires. Paris, 1–23.Google Scholar
  64. Marler, P., & Hamilton, W. J. (1966). Mechanisms of animal behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  65. Müller, F. M. (1881). Chips from a German workshop volume 4. New York: C. Scribner’s sons.Google Scholar
  66. Nishimura, T. (2008). Understanding the dynamics of primate vocalization and its implications for the evolution of human speech. In The origins of language (pp. 111–131). Tokoyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Nishimura, T., Mikami, A., Suzuki, J., & Matsuzawa, T. (2006). Descent of the hyoid in chimpanzees: evolution of face flattening and speech. Journal of Human Evolution, 51(3), 244–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Nishimura, T., Oishi, T., Suzuki, J., Matsuda, K., & Takahashi, T. (2008). Development of the supralaryngeal vocal tract in Japanese macaques: implications for the evolution of the descent of the larynx. American journal of physical anthropology, 135(2), 182–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Pagliaro, A. (1961). Lingua e poesia secondo GB Vico. In A. Pagliaro, Altri saggi di critica semantica, D’Anna: Messina, 307 et seq.Google Scholar
  70. Parchappe, J. B. (1865). Discussion sur la faculté du langage articulé. Bulletin de l’Académie Impériale de Médecine, 679–703.Google Scholar
  71. Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(02), 109–130.Google Scholar
  72. Pennisi, A. (1987). La linguistica dei mercatanti: Filosofia linguistica e filosofia civile da Vico a Cuoco. Napoli: Guida.Google Scholar
  73. Pennisi, A. (1990). “Numerare” et “discorrere” dans la tradition italienne du verum-factum. In D. Di Cesare & S. Gensini (Eds.), Iter Babelicum, Studien zur Historiographie der Linguistik. 1600-1800 (pp. 53–68). Münster: Nodus.Google Scholar
  74. Pennisi, A. (1993). Ingenium e patologie del linguaggio: su alcune fonti della linguistica vichiana. In D. Droixhe & C. Grell (Eds.), La linguistique entre mythe et histoire (pp. 667–704). Münster: Nodus.Google Scholar
  75. Pennisi, A. (1994). Le lingue mutole: Le patologie del linguaggio fra teoria e storia. Roma: NIS.Google Scholar
  76. Pennisi, A. (2014a). L’errore di Platone. In Biopolitica, linguaggio e diritti civili in tempi di crisi. Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
  77. Pennisi, A. (2014b). La tecnologia del linguaggio tra passato e presente. Blitiry, II(2), 195–220.Google Scholar
  78. Pennisi, A., & Giallongo, L. (2015). Come votano gli animali. Un contributo di etica naturalistica per una nuova idea di biopolitica. In G. Airenti, M. Cruciani, S. Di Nuovo, P. Perconti, & A. Plebe (Eds.), Le scienze cognitive a confronto. Oltre i confini della teoria (pp. 233–244). Roma-Messina: Corisco.Google Scholar
  79. Perrault, C. (1676). Mémoires pour servir à l’histoire naturelle des animaux. Paris: L’Imprimerie Royale.Google Scholar
  80. Radick, G. (2000). Language, brain function, and human origins in the Victorian debates on evolution. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 31(1), 55–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Seeley, T. D. (2009). The wisdom of the hive: The social physiology of honey bee colonies. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Seeley, T. D. (2010). Honeybee democracy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2010). Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain and language, 115(1), 92–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Smith, C. U. M. (2013). Cardiocentric neurophysiology: The persistence of a delusion. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 22(1), 6–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Solinas, M. (2015). From Aristotle’s teleology to Darwin’s genealogy: The stamp of inutility. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Stanford, C. B., & Bunn, H. T. (Eds.). (2001). Meat-eating and human evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Stephen, L. (1873). Essays on freethinking and plain speaking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2012).Google Scholar
  88. Tipton, J. A. (2014). Philosophical biology in Aristotle’s parts of animals. Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Tyson, E. (1699). Orang-outang, sive Homo sylvestris. London: Thomas Bennet & Daniel Brown.Google Scholar
  90. Ungar, P. S. (2007). Evolution of the human diet: The known, the unknown, and the unknowable. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  91. Verene, D. P. (1981). Vico’s science of imagination. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  92. Vico, G. (AC) In Croce, B., & In Nicolini, F. (Eds.), L’autobiografia: Il carteggio e le poesie varie. Bari: G. Laterza (1929). Tr. ing. In Villarosa, C. A. R., Fisch, M. H., & Bergin, T. G. (1944). The autobiography of Giambattista Vico. Ithaca: Cornell University Press (1994)Google Scholar
  93. Vico, G. (AIS). De antiquissima Italorum Sapientia ex linguae latinae originibus eruenda (1710). In Vico (OF). pp. 55–131.Google Scholar
  94. Vico, G. (DS). De nostris temporis studiorum ratione (1708). In Vico (OF). pp. 788–855.Google Scholar
  95. Vico, G. (OR). On humanistic education: (six inaugural orations, 1699–1707). In Visconti G. G (Ed.), Ithaca: Cornell University Press (1993).Google Scholar
  96. Von Kempelen, W. (1778) Mechanismus der menschlichen sprache: nebst der Beschreibung seiner sprechenden Maschine, Wien: Degen (1791).Google Scholar
  97. Whitney, W. D. (1867). Language, and the study of language: Twelve lectures on the principles of linguistic science. New York: C. Scribner & Company.Google Scholar
  98. Wrangham, R. W. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human. Basic Books.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Antonino Pennisi
    • 1
  • Alessandra Falzone
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Cognitive ScienceUniversity of MessinaMessinaItaly

Personalised recommendations