Advertisement

Chomsky and Biolinguistics

  • Antonino Pennisi
  • Alessandra Falzone
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology book series (PEPRPHPS, volume 12)

Abstract

This chapter describes the origin of contemporary biolinguistics. In particular, the authors discuss the philosophical and linguistic presuppositions of Chomskyan biolinguistics and his ambiguous positions on dualism and Platonism. They also analyze the Chomskyan approach to evolutionism and its criticism of the more functionalist and adaptationist aspects of Darwinism. They highlight his choice to counteract the tendency of naturalistic studies not to develop a solid theory of language.

Keywords

Human Language Language Faculty Cognitive Ethology Functionalist Reading Contemporary Cognitive Science 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Arbib, M. A. (2002). The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In C. Nehaniv & K. Dautenhahn (Eds.), Imitation in animals and artifacts. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  2. Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105–167.Google Scholar
  3. Arbib, M. A. (2006). Action to language via the mirror neuron system. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language. The mirror system hypothesis. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053–1076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us. Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace and World, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chomsky, N. (1972). Studies on semantics in generative grammar. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  9. Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
  10. Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York/Oxford: Columbia University Press/Basil Blackwell Publisher.Google Scholar
  11. Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and the problems of knowledge: The managua lectures. Cambridge, MA/London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  12. Chomsky, N. (1993a). Language and thought. Wakefield: Moyer Bell.Google Scholar
  13. Chomsky, N. (2000a). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chomsky, N. (2002). In A. Belletti, L. Rizzi & N. Chomsky (2002). On nature and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic inquiry, 36(1), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chomsky, N. (2007). Biolinguistic explorations: Design, development, evolution. International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 15(1), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chomsky, N. (2011). Language and other cognitive systems. What is special about language? Language Learning and Development, 7(4), 263–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Chomsky, N. (2013). Problems of projection. Lingua, 130, 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Corballis, M. C. (1999). Cerebral asymmetry: Motoring on. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 152–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth. Princeton: Princeton Univerisity Press.Google Scholar
  21. Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of man and selection in relation to sex (1st ed.). London: John Murray [Cambdrige University Press. 2009].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Donald, M. (2005). Imitation and mimesis. In S. L. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals (pp. 283–300). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Ferretti, F. (2010). Alle origini del linguaggio umano: il punto di vista evoluzionistico. Roma: Laterza.Google Scholar
  24. Ferretti, F. (2014). Travelling in time and space at the origins of language. Humana. Mente–Journal of Philosophical Studies, 27, 243–268.Google Scholar
  25. Ferretti, F. (2015). La facoltà di linguaggio. Determinanti biologiche e variabilità culturale. Roma: Carocci.Google Scholar
  26. Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97(2), 179–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gentilucci, M., & Corballis, M. C. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(7), 949–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gould, S. J. (2002). The structure of evolutionary theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hauser, M. D., Yang, C., Berwick, R. C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M. J., Watumull, J., et al. (2014). The mystery of language evolution. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 401.Google Scholar
  31. Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  32. Lorenz, K. (1983). Der Abbau des Menschlichen. München: Piper.Google Scholar
  33. Meguerditchian, A., & Vauclair, J. (2008). Vocal and gestural communication in nonhuman primates and the question of the origin of language. Learning from animals, 61–85.Google Scholar
  34. Meguerditchian, A., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Investigation of gestural vs vocal origins of language in nonhuman primates: Distinguishing comprehension and production of signals. In S. A., S. M., & S. K. de Boer (Eds.), The evolution of language (pp. 453–454). Singapore: World Scientific.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mithen, S. J. (2006). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind, and body. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(02), 109–130.Google Scholar
  37. Pennisi, A. (2014a). L’errore di Platone. In Biopolitica, linguaggio e diritti civili in tempi di crisi. Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
  38. Pennisi, A. (2014b). La tecnologia del linguaggio tra passato e presente. Blitiry, II(2), 195–220.Google Scholar
  39. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  41. Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zlatev, J. (2008). From proto-mimesis to language: Evidence from primatology and social neuroscience. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), 137–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Antonino Pennisi
    • 1
  • Alessandra Falzone
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Cognitive ScienceUniversity of MessinaMessinaItaly

Personalised recommendations