Skip to main content

Bio-Linguistic Plasticity and Origin of Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Darwinian Biolinguistics

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology ((PEPRPHPS,volume 12))

  • 400 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter is devoted to the application of knowledge on functional and, in particular, evolutionary plasticity, these being two of the most important issues concerning biolinguistics. On the basis of the previous chapter, we have summarized a scheme consisting of four levels of evolutionary innovation processes: (1) the level of the “mechanical trigger" of innovation; (2) the level of performative innovation compatibility in the context of evolutionary development of the structure; (3) the level of natural selection within population genetics; (4) the level of adaptation and cultural selection. These four levels are considered in subsections on the origin of language, the development of historical-natural languages, language selection, the mechanisms of adaptation/maladaptation of cultural products and possible (but not certain) developmental outcomes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ackermann, H., Hage, S. R., & Ziegler, W. (2014). Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: An evolutionary perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 529–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiello, L., & Dean, C. (2002). An introduction to human evolutionary anatomy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aitchison, J. (2006). The Articulate Mammal. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alroy, J. (1998). Equilibrial diversity dynamics in North American Mammals. In M. L. McKinney & J. Drake (Eds.), (pp 232–287). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ankel-Simon, F. (2007). Primate anatomy. An introduction. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2002). The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In C. Nehaniv & K. Dautenhahn (Eds.), Imitation in animals and artifacts. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2006). Action to language via the mirror neuron system. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language. The mirror system hypothesis. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (2016). Your soul is a distributed property of the brains of yourself and others, RSL. Italian Journal of Cognitive Science, 3(1), 5–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053–1076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, D. F. (1999). Original signs: Gesture, sign, and the sources of language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, D. F., & Wilcox, S. E. (2007). The gestural origin of language. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., & Wilcox, S. E. (1994). Signs of the origin of syntax. Current Anthropology, 35(4), 349–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use. Forms of engagement in human and animal use of tools. London: Taylor and Francis.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M., Allen, C., & Burghardt, M. (2002). The cognitive animal. Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belyk, M., & Brown, S. (2014). Somatotopy of the extrinsic laryngeal muscles in the human sensorimotor cortex. Behavioural and Brain Research, 270, 364–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benton, M. J. (2002). Cope’s rule. In M. Pagel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution (pp. 209–210). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us. Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (2003). Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution. In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 77–93). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (2014). More than nature need. Language, mind, and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Birkhead, T. R., & Møller, A. P. (1998). Sperm competition and sexual selection. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biro, D., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001). Chimpanzee numerical competence: Cardinal and ordinal skills. In T. Matsuzawa (Ed.), Primate origins of human cognition and behavior (pp. 199–225). Springer: Tokyo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boë, L. J., Maeda, S., & Heim, J.-L. (1999). Neanderthal man was not morphologically handicapped for speech. Evolution of Communication, 3, 49–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boë, L. J., Heim, J. L., Honda, K., & Maeda, S. (2002). The potential Neandertal vowel space was as large as that of modern humans. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 465–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boë, L. J., Heim, J. L., Honda, K., Maeda, S., Badin, P., & Abry, C. (2007). The vocal tract of newborn humans and Neanderthals: Acoustic capabilities and consequences for the debate on the origin of language. A reply to Lieberman (2007a). Journal of Phonetics, 35, 564–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C. (2011). Biolinguistics: A brief guide for the perplexed. Language Science, 5, 449–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, D. (2013). The nature and origin of language. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S., Ngan, E., & Liotti, M. (2008). A larynx area in the human motor cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18(4), 837–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S., Laird, A. R., Pfordresher, P. Q., Thelen, S. M., Turkeltaub, P., & Liotti, M. (2009). The somatotopy of speech. Phonation and articulation in the human motor cortex. Brain and Cognition, 70, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartei, V., & Reby, D. (2013). Effect of formant frequency spacing on perceived gender in pre-pubertal children’s voices. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e81022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartei, V., Wind Cowles, H., & Reby, D. (2012). Spontaneous voice gender imitation abilities in adult speakers. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e31353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information? Behaviour, 94(1/2), 150–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Why animals don’t have language. The Tanner Lectures on Human Values, 19, 173–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2005). Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. The Linguistic Review, 22, 135–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008a). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008b). The language faculty that wasn’t: A usage-based account of natural language recursion. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1182), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2003a). Language evolution: Consensus and controversies. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 300–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2003b). Language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1999). Cerebral asymmetry: Motoring on. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 152–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth. Princeton: Princeton Univerisity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2003). Out on a Limb: A reply to Bradshaw’s review of from hand to mouth. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 8(2), 195–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2015). The wandering mind. What the brain does when you’re not looking. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild Chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, E. T., & Sawchenko, P. E. (2000). Dorsal medullary pathways subserving oromotor reflexes in the rat: Implications for the central neural control of swallowing. Journal Comparative Neurology, 417, 448–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, T. M. (2003). The great leap forward: The anatomic basis for the acquisition of speech and obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Medicine, 4, 185–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Mauro, T. (1980). Minisemantica. Roma: Laterza.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1989). The neural circuitry underlying primate calls and human language. Journal of Human Evolution, 4, 367–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species. The co-evolution of language and the human brain. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (2003). Universal grammar and semiotic constraints. In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 111–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Diogo, R., & Wood, B. (2012). Violation of Dollo’s law. Evidence of muscle reversions in primate phylogeny and their implications for the understanding of the ontogeny, evolution, and anatomical variations of modern humans. Evolution, 66(10), 3267–3276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2005). Imitation and mimesis. In S. L. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals (pp. 283–300). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of Language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Brain, 9(10), 178–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317(5843), 1344–1347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. C., Halenar, L. B., Davies, T. G., Cristobal-Azkarate, J., Reby, D., Sykes, D., et al. (2015). Evolutionary trade-off between vocal tract and testes dimensions in howler monkeys. Current Biology, 25(21), 2839–2844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire. On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falzone, A. (2012). Evoluzionismo e comunicazione. Nuove ipotesi sulla selezione naturale nei linguaggi animali e umani. Roma: Corisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, D. R. (2008). Are human faces and voices ornaments signaling common underlying cues to mate value? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 17, 112–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti, F. (2007). Perché non siamo speciali. Mente, linguaggio e natura umana. Laterza: Roma-Bari.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti, F. (2010). Alle origini del linguaggio umano: il punto di vista evoluzionistico. Roma: Laterza.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 258–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukushima, M., Saunders, R.C., Fujii, N., Averbeck, B.B., & Mishkin, M. (2014). Modeling vocalization with ECoG cortical activity recorded during vocal production in the macaque monkey. In 36th Annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society, IEEE, (pp. 6794–6797).

    Google Scholar 

  • Galis, F., Arntzen, J. W., & Lande, R. (2010). Dollo’s law and the irreversibility of digit loss in Bachia. Evolution, 64(8), 2466–2476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamba, M. (2014). Vocal tract-related cues across human and nonhuman signals. RSL. Italian Journal of Cognitive Science, 1(1), 49–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamba, M., Friard, O., & Giacoma, C. (2012). Vocal tract morphology determines species-specific features in vocal signals of lemurs (Eulemur). International Journal of Primatology, 33, 1453–1466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaston, K. J., Blackburn, T. M., & Lawton, J. H. (1998). Aggregation and the interspecific abundance-occupancy relationship. Journal of Animal Ecology, 67, 995–999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., & Corballis, M. C. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(7), 949–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genty, E., Clay, Z., Hobaiter, C., & Zuberbühler, K. (2014). Multi-modal use of a socially directed call in bonobos. PloS one, 9(1), e84738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghazanfar, A. A., & Rendall, D. (2008). Evolution of human vocal production. Current Biology, 18(11), R457–R460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, E. E., & Igić, B. (2008). On phylogenetic tests of irreversible evolution. Evolution, 62(11), 2727–2741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. C. (2004). Apes monkeys children and the growth of mind the developing child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, S. I., & Shanker, S. G. (2004). The first idea. How symbols, language and intelligence evolved from our primate ancestors to modern humans. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hage, S. R., & Nieder, A. (2013). Single neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex encode volitional initiation of vocalizations. Nature Communications, 4, 2409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hage, S. R., & Nieder, A. (2015). Audio-vocal interaction in single neurons of the monkey ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(18), 7030–7040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 14, 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickok, G. (2014). The myth of mirror neurons: The real neuroscience of communication and cognition. New York: WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hone, D. W., & Benton, M. J. (2005). The evolution of large size: How does Cope’s rule work? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20(1), 4–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D. (Ed.). (2007). The evolution of hemispheric specialization in primates (Vol. 5). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., & Leavens, D. A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal behaviour, 73(2), 281–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S. M., Farley, S. D., & Rhodes, B. C. (2010). Vocal and physiological changes in response to the physical attractiveness of conversational partners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(3), 155–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S. M., Mogilski, J. K., & Harrison, M. A. (2014). The perception and parameters of intentional voice manipulation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38(1), 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. R. (2008). Niche-construction, co-evolution, and domain-specificity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(05), 526–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. R. (2014). Origins of language: A slim guide. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. L., & Nudds, M. (2006). Rational animals? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2008). The role of premotor cortex in speech perception: Evidence from fmri and rtms. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), 31–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iwatsubo, T., Kuzuhara, S., Kanemitsu, A., Shimada, H., & Toyokura, Y. (1990). Corticofugal projections to the motor nuclei of the brain stem and spinal cord in humans. Neurology, 40, 309–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jürgens, U. (1974). On the elicitability of vocalization from the cortical larynx area. Brain Research, 81, 564–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jürgens, U. (2009). The neural control of vocalization in mammals: A review. Journal of Voice, 23(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jürgens, U., Kirzinger, A., & Von Cramon, D. (1982). The effects of deep-reaching lesions in the cortical face area on phonation a combined case report and experimental monkey study. Cortex, 18(1), 125–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science, 304, 1682–1683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, V., Puts, D. A., & Cárdenas, R. A. (2013). Masculine men articulate less clearly. Human Nature, 24(4), 461–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2002). Historical observations on the relationship between research on sign languages and language origins theory. In D. F. Armstrong, M. Karchmer, & J. V. Van Cleve (Eds.), The study of signed languages: Essays in honor of William Stokoe (pp. 13–34). Washington: Gallaudet University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kingsolver, J. G., & Pfennig, D. W. (2004). Individual-level selection as a cause of Cope’s rule of phyletic size increase. Evolution, 58(7), 1608–1612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (1999). Function, selection, and innateness: The emergence of language universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2002). Learning, Bottlenecks and the Evolution of Recursive Syntax. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models (pp. 173–203). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Klofstad, C. A., Anderson, R. C., & Nowicki, S. (2015). Perceptions of competence, strength, and age influence voters to select leaders with lower-pitched voices. PloS one, 10(8), e0133779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota, S. (2001). The extinction program for Homo sapiens and cloning humans: Trinucleotide expansion as a one-way track to extinction. Medical hypotheses, 56(3), 296–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190(4209), 69–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1958). Corticobulbar connexions to the pons and lower brain-stem in man. Brain, 81(3), 364–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Kowalsky, B., de Vries, H., Spruijt, B. M., Sterck, E. H., et al. (2013). Orangutan (Pongo spp.) whistling and implications for the emergence of an open-ended call repertoire: A replication and extension. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(3), 2326–2335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Bartlett, A. M., Shumaker, R. W., Wich, S. A., & Menken, S. B. (2015). Speech-like rhythm in a voiced and voiceless orangutan call. PloS one, 10(1), e116136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laporte, M. N., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour, 80(3), 467–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leongómez, J. D., Binter, J., Kubicová, L., Stolařová, P., Klapilová, K., Havlíček, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2014). Vocal modulation during courtship increases proceptivity even in naive listeners. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(6), 489–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1955). Tristes tropiques. Paris: PLON.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication. Origins and implications for language processing. Trends in cognitive sciences, 20(1), 6–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (2008). A wild 50,000-year ride. InHot pursuit of language in prehistory. Essays in the four fields of anthropology in honor of Harold Crane Fleming (pp. 359–371). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (2012). Vocal tract anatomy and the neural bases of talking. Journal of Phonetics, 40(4), 608–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., & McCarthy, R. (2007). Tracking the evolution of language and speech: Comparing vocal tracts to identify speech capabilities. Expedition: The magazine of the University of Pennsylvania, 49(2), 15–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., & Matsuzawa, T. (2010). The mind of the Chimpanzee. Ecological and experimental perspectives. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1973a). Die acht Todsünden der zivilisierten Menschheit. München: Piper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1983). Der Abbau des Menschlichen. München: Piper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loucks, T. M., Poletto, C. J., Simonyan, K., Reynolds, C. L., & Ludlow, C. L. (2007). Human brain activation during phonation and exhalation: Common volitional control for two upper airway functions. Neuroimage, 36(1), 131–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2008). Cognitive precursors to language. In Oller, D. K., & Griebel, U. (2008), 193–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, J. (2003). What it means to be 98% chimpanzee: Apes, people, and their genes. Univ of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T. (2007). Comparative cognitive development. Developmental science, 10(1), 97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T., Tomonaga, M., & Tanaka, M. (2006). Cognitive development in chimpanzees. Tokyo: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meguerditchian, A., & Vauclair, J. (2008). Vocal and gestural communication in nonhuman primates and the question of the origin of language. Learning from animals, 61–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meguerditchian, A., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Investigation of gestural vs vocal origins of language in nonhuman primates: Distinguishing comprehension and production of signals. In S. A., S. M., & S. K. de Boer (Eds.), The evolution of language (pp. 453–454). Singapore: World Scientific.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Milojevic, B., & Hast, M. H. (1964). Cortical motor centers of the Laryngeal muscles in the Cat and Dog. The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology, 73, 979–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. J. (2006). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind, and body. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H., & Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: A two-country study. Acta Paediatrica, 102(2), 156–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24(1), 45–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, G. B., & Wagner, G. P. (1991). Novelty in evolution: restructuring the concept. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 22, 229–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J. J., Pisanski, K., Tigue, C. C., Fraccaro, P. J., & Feinberg, D. R. (2014). Perceptions of infidelity risk predict women’s preferences for low male voice pitch in short-term over long-term relationship contexts. Personality and Individual differences, 56, 73–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagel, M. (2009). Human language as a culturally transmitted replicator. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10(6), 405–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagel, M. D. (2012). Wired for culture: The natural history of human cooperation. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paget, R. (1930). Human speech: Some observations, experiments, and conclusions as to the nature, origin, purpose and possible improvement of human speech. London/New York: K. Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., Ltd/Harcourt, Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain-mechanisms. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennisi, A. (2014a). L’errore di Platone. InBiopolitica, linguaggio e diritti civili in tempi di crisi. Bologna: Il Mulino.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennisi, A. (2014b). La tecnologia del linguaggio tra passato e presente. Blitiry, II(2), 195–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennisi, A., & Falzone, A. (2010). Il prezzo del linguaggio. Evoluzione ed estinzione nelle scienze cognitive. Bologna: Il Mulino.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, R. H. (1983). The Ecological Implications of Body Size. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pisanski, K., & Bryant, G. A. (2016). The evolution of voice perception. In N. S. Eidsheim & K. L. Meizel (Eds.), The. Oxford Handbook of Voice Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisanski, K., Cartei, V., McGettigan, C., Raine, J., & Reby, D. (2016). Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends in cognitive sciences, 20(4), 304–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and brain sciences, 1(04), 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preuss, T. M. (2004). What is it like to be human? In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences III (pp. 5–22). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rödel, R. M., Olthoff, A., Tergau, F., Simonyan, K., Kraemer, D., Markus, H., & Kruse, E. (2004). Human cortical motor representation of the larynx as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). The Laryngoscope, 114(5), 918–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K., & Slocombe, K. E. (2013). Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS One, 8(10), e76674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenemann, P. T. (1999). Syntax as an emergent characteristic of the evolution of semantic complexity. Minds and Machines, 9(3), 309–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2010). Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain and language, 115(1), 92–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., & Horwitz, B. (2011). Laryngeal motor cortex and control of speech in humans. The Neuroscientist, 2, 197–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., & Jürgens, U. (2002). Cortico-cortical projections of the motorcortical larynx area in the rhesus monkey. Brain research, 949(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., & Jürgens, U. (2005a). Afferent cortical connections of the motor cortical larynx area in the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience, 130(1), 133–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., & Jürgens, U. (2005b). Afferent subcortical connections into the motor cortical larynx area in the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience, 130(1), 119–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonyan, K., Ostuni, J., Ludlow, C. L., & Horwitz, B. (2009). Functional but not structural networks of the human laryngeal motor cortex show left hemispheric lateralization during syllable but not breathing production. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(47), 14912–14923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. B. (2007). Marine diversity through the phanerozoic: Problems and prospects. Journal of the Geological Society, 164(4), 731–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, J., Ferrari, P. F., & Fogassi, L. (2012). From action to language: Comparative perspectives on primate tool use, gesture and the evolution of human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 4–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, K. N. (1972). The quantal nature of speech: Evidence from articulatory acoustic data. In E. E. David Jr. & P. B. Denes (Eds.), Human communication: A unified view (pp. 51–66). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, D., Larson, C., & Lindeman, R. C. (1974). Neocortical and limbic lesion effects on primate phonation. Brain research, 71(1), 61–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Baker, L. A. (2003). Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology, 24(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallerman, M. (2014). No syntax saltation in language evolution. Language Sciences, 46, 207–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. (2009). Not a chimp: The hunt to find the genes that make us human. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. K. (1995). Natural and relational concepts in animals. Comparative approaches to cognitive science, 175, 224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2011). Do chimpanzees know what others see – Or only what they are looking at? In Hurley & Nudds (Eds.), (pp 541–565).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Zuberbühler, K. (2002). Primate vocal and gestural communication. In M. Beckoff, C. S. Allen, & G. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 293–299). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E. A., & Zentall, T. R. (2006). Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (2012). The social conquest of earth. New York: Liveright Pub. Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. (2001). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour, 61(6), 1203–1216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Sereno, M. I., & Iacoboni, M. (2004). Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature neuroscience, 7(7), 701–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour, 144(12), 1621–1653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1900). Die Sprache (Vol. 2). Leipzig: Enghelman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (2007). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. New York-Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S., & Nash, J. (1999). Inner vision: An exploration of art and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J. (2008). From proto-mimesis to language: Evidence from primatology and social neuroscience. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), 137–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler, K. (2005). The phylogenetic roots of language. Evidence from primate. Communication and Cognition, 14(3), 126–130.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pennisi, A., Falzone, A. (2016). Bio-Linguistic Plasticity and Origin of Language. In: Darwinian Biolinguistics . Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 12. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47688-9_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47688-9_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-47686-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-47688-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics