Advertisement

Evolutionary Plasticity

  • Antonino Pennisi
  • Alessandra Falzone
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology book series (PEPRPHPS, volume 12)

Abstract

Understanding evolutionary plasticity is one of the key points of the DBM. While, in fact, the finding of functional plasticity fails to shed light on the relationships and hierarchies among externalization devices and central coordination arrangements due to the substantial simultaneity with which they occur, the evolutionary approach allows us to do just that. In an orthodox Darwinian framework – respectful of the structural gradualness principle – it seems clear that the neuromotor coordination processes must necessarily follow the innovation (mutation) structural process. This principle is reconstructed through the pioneering path of Leroy-Gourhan and formalized through the current research of the Evo-Devo on evolutionary innovation theory.

Keywords

Autism Spectrum Disorder Innovation Theory Evolutionary Innovation Brain Plasticity Selective Adaptation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Allen, J. S. (2009). The lives of the brain: Human evolution and the organ of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, M. L., & Penner-Wilger, M. (2013). Neural reuse in the evolution and development of the brain: Evidence for developmental Homology? Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 42–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bergson, H. (1896) Matter and Memory (1896) (N. M. Paul & W. Scott Palmer, Trans.). London: George Allen and Unwin (1911).Google Scholar
  4. Bergson, H. (1908). Memory of the present and false recognition. In Bergson 1919 (pp. 134–185).Google Scholar
  5. Bergson, H. (1911). Life and consciousness. In Bergson 1919 (pp. 3–36).Google Scholar
  6. Bergson, H. (1912). The soul and the body. In Bergson 1919 (pp. 37–74).Google Scholar
  7. Bergson, H. (1913). “Phantasms of the living” and psychical research. In Bergson 1919 (pp. 75–103).Google Scholar
  8. Bergson, H. (1919). Mind-energy: Lectures and essays. Westport: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
  9. Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us. Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  10. Binswanger, L. (1956). Drei Formen Missglückten Dasein. Verlag: Tübingen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Blankenburg, W. (1971). Der Verlust der naturlichen Selbstverstandlichkeit. Stuttgart: Verlag.Google Scholar
  12. Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N., & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How could language have evolved? PLoS Biology, 12(8).Google Scholar
  13. Churchland, P. S. (1979). Scientific realism and the plasticity of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clowry, G. J. (2014). Seeking clues in brain development to explain the extraordinary evolution of language in humans. Language Sciences, 46, 220–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Corballis, M. C. (1993). The Lopsided Ape. Evolution of the generative mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dart, R. A. (1959). Adventures with the missing link. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  17. Dehaene, S. (2011). Education as cortical recycling: The case of reading and math. Global Education, 2011-09.Google Scholar
  18. Etkin, W. (1954). Social behavior and the evolution of man’s mental faculties. American Naturalist, 129–142.Google Scholar
  19. Freeman, W. J. (2000). How brains make up their minds. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  20. García-Moreno, F., Vasistha, N. A., Trevia, N., Bourne, J. A., & Molnár, Z. (2011). Compartmentalization of cerebral cortical germinal zones in a lissencephalic primate and gyrencephalic rodent. Cerebral Cortex, bhr312.Google Scholar
  21. Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). Human. The science behind what makes us unique. Harper-Collins ebook.Google Scholar
  22. Ghazanfar, A. A., & Rendall, D. (2008). Evolution of human vocal production. Current Biology, 18(11), R457–R460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). San Marco and the panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 205(1161), 581–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hewes, G. W. (1961). Food transport and the origin of hominid bipedalism. American Anthropologist, 63(4), 687–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hunt, K. D. (1994). The evolution of human bipedality: ecology and functional morphology. Journal of Human Evolution, 26(3), 183–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hurford, J. R. (2008). Niche-construction, co-evolution, and domain-specificity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(05), 526–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jolly, C. J. (1970). The seed-eaters: A new model for hominoid differentiation based on a baboon analogy. Man, 5, 1–26.Google Scholar
  29. Kirschmann, E. (1999). Das Zeitalter der Werfer—eine neue Sicht des Menschen. Das Schimpansen-Werfer-Aasfresser.Google Scholar
  30. LeDoux, J. (2002). Synaptic self: How brains become who we are. New York: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
  31. Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lieberman, P. (2013). The unpredictable species. What makes humans unique. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lorenz, K. (1959). Psychologie und Stammesgeschichte. In G. Heberer (Ed.), Evolution der Organismen (pp. 131–172). Stuttgart: Fischer.Google Scholar
  34. Love, A. C. (2015). Conceptual change in biology. Scientific and philosophical perspectives on evolution and development. Dordecht: Springer.Google Scholar
  35. Malabou, C. (1995). The future of Hegel: Plasticity, temporality, and dialectic. New York: Routledge (2005).Google Scholar
  36. Malabou, C., & Shread, C. P. T. (2012). Ontology of the accident: An essay on destructive plasticity. Cambridge, UK: Polity.Google Scholar
  37. Mayr, E. (1960). The emergence of evolutionary novelties. Evolution after Darwin, 1, 349–380.Google Scholar
  38. Mayr, E. (1963). Animal species and evolution. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Minelli, A., & Fusco, G. (2005). Conserved versus innovative features in animal body organization. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 304(6), 520–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Minkowski, E. (1966). Traité de psychopathologie. Paris: Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  41. Minkowski, E. (1997). Au-delà du rationalisme morbide. Paris: Éditions L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
  42. Muller, G. B., & Wagner, G. P. (1991). Novelty in evolution: restructuring the concept. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 22, 229–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Newman, S. A., & Müller, G. B. (2005). Origination and innovation in the vertebrate limb skeleton: an epigenetic perspective. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 304(6), 593–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Niemitz, C. (2010). The evolution of the upright posture and gait. A review and a new synthesis. Naturwissenschaften, 97(3), 241–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral cortex of man: A clinical study of localization of function. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  46. Pennisi, A. (1998). Psicopatologia del linguaggio: Storia, analisi, filosofie della mente. Roma: Carocci.Google Scholar
  47. Pennisi, A. (2001). Langage, action, realité chez H. Bergson. In G. D. & D. Chiricò (Eds.), Henri Bergson: ésprit et langage (pp. 147–161). Liège: Mardaga.Google Scholar
  48. Pennisi, A. (2012b). Diete per la mente. Ovvero: come astenersi dalle analogie e combatterne la pervasività per evitare la malattia mentale e la morte della creatività. In D. R. Hofstadter, Sulla coscienza, la creatività e il pensiero analogico, Pennisi & P. Perconti (eds.), Roma-Messina: Corisco, (pp. 113–137).Google Scholar
  49. Platek, S. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2009). Foundations in evolutionary cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rakic, P. (2009). Evolution of the neocortex: A perspective from developmental biology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(10), 724–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rose, M. D. (1976). Bipedal behavior of olive baboons (Papio anubis) and its relevance to an understanding of the evolution of human bipedalism. American journal of physical anthropology, 44(2), 247–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rose, M. R., & Lauder, G. V. (1996). Adaptation. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  53. Skoyles, J. R., & Sagan, D. (2002). Up from dragons: The evolution of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  54. Solinas, M. (2015). From Aristotle’s teleology to Darwin’s genealogy: The stamp of inutility. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wagner, A. (2011). The origins of evolutionary innovations: A theory of transformative change in living systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wagner, G. P. (2014). Homology, genes, and evolutionary innovation. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Washburn, S.L. (1967). Behaviour and the origin of man. Proceedings of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 1967, 21-27.Google Scholar
  58. West-Eberhard, M. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Wrangham, R. W. (1980). Bipedal locomotion as a feeding adaptation in gelada baboons, and its implications for hominid evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 9(4), 329–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Young, R. W. (2003). Evolution of the human hand: The role of throwing and clubbing. Journal of Anatomy, 202(1), 165–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Antonino Pennisi
    • 1
  • Alessandra Falzone
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Cognitive ScienceUniversity of MessinaMessinaItaly

Personalised recommendations