Skip to main content

Abstract

This volume explores methods used by social scientists and human biologists to understand fundamental aspects of human experience. We are interested in phenomena experienced by individuals that are often “invisible” to one’s self or others. Some of these phenomena are intricately associated with cognitive processes so that social scientists must, in a sense, get into the heads of other people to understand how they experience the world. This involves verbal and nonverbal communication, or an examination of biological processes that underlie the cognitive activity involved with the experience. Other “invisible” phenomena are not usually associated with subjective experience but can be made visible to individuals through objective measures. Biological markers have taken on importance as tools for enhancing measurement and our understanding of the life experiences of others. This book is organized by stages of the human life span: beginnings, adulthood, and aging. We explore how we learn about particular kinds of experience—including pain, stress, activity levels, sleep quality, memory, and menopausal hot flashes—that have traditionally relied upon self-reports, but are subject to inter-individual differences in self-awareness or culture-based notions of what experiences to note or report. We also examine other ways in which normally “invisible” phenomena can be made visible, such as the caloric content of foods, blood pressure, early life experiences, growth, nutritional status, and bone health. The use of biological markers to understand human experience is one of the most difficult, exciting, and important areas of scientific research into the human condition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Amundson, R. (2016). Subjective and objective knowledge: Persistent puzzles. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 303–309). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, D. J. P., Hales, C. N., Fall, C. H. D., Osmond, C., Phipps, K., & Clark, P. M. S. (1993). Type 2 (non-insulin dependent) diabetes mellitus, hypertension and hyperlipidaemia (syndrome X): Relation to reduced fetal growth. Diabetologia, 36, 62–67. doi:10.1007/BF00399095

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baucom, L. B., Wedell, D. H., Wang, J., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2012). Decoding the neural representation of affective states. Neuroimage, 59, 718–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beach, F. A. (1975). Behavioral endocrinology: An emerging discipline. American Scientist, 63, 178–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brener, N. D., Billy, J. O. G., & Grady, W. R. (2003). Assessment of factors affecting the validity of self-reported health-risk behavior among adolescents: Evidence from the scientific literature. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33, 436–457. doi:10.1016/S1054-139X(03)00052-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. E. (2016). Stress biomarkers as an objective window on experience. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 117–141). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. G., Calibuso, M. J., & Roedl, A. L. (2011). Women’s sexuality, well-being, and the menstrual cycle: Methodological issues and their interrelationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 755–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, Jl. (2003). Hormonal mediation of physiological and behavioral processes that influence fertility. In K. W. Wachter & R. A. Bulatao (Eds.), Offspring: Human fertility behavior in biodemographic perspective (pp. 104–139). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C. S., Boone, E., Grippo, A. J., Ruscio, M., & Bales, K. L. (2009). The endocrinology of social relationships in rodents. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 121–137). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chikazoe, J., Lee, D. H., Kriegeskorte, N., & Anderson, A. K. (2014). Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 1114–1122. doi:10.1038/nn.3749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1980). The face of man: Expressions of universal emotions in a New Guinea village. New York: Garland STPM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellison, P. T. (2003). Energetics and reproductive effort. American Journal of Human Biology, 15(3), 342–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferris, C. F., Axelson, J. F., Shinto, L. H., & Albers, H. E. (1987). Scent marking and the maintenance of dominant/subordinate status in male golden hamsters. Physiology & Behavior, 40, 661–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, W. I., & Thurston, R. C. (2016). Hot flashes: Phenomenology and measurement. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 233–254). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D., Krieger, H., & Wong, P. Y. (1997). Hormonal and experiential correlates of maternal responsiveness during pregnancy and the puerperium in human mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 145–158. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1997.1376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, L. M., Giambrone, A. E., Al-Ali, H. M., Verjee, M. A. (2015). Validity of self-reported vitamin-D deficiency among midlife Arab women living in Qatar. American Journal of Human Biology, 28, 181–185. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22766

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, L. M., & Madimenos, F. C. (2016). Bone health in midlife women. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 255–273). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hales, C. N., & Barker, D. J. P. (2001). The thrifty phenotype hypothesis. British Medical Bulletin, 60, 5–20. doi:10.1093/bmb/60.1.5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, S. M. (1987). Female sexual behavior: Fluctuations during the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31, 101–110. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(87)90104-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, M. W., & Robinson, M. E. (2016). The challenge of measuring pain in humans. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 105–115). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (1993). Life history theory and evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 2, 78–88. doi:10.1002/evan.1360020303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, G. D. (2016). Continuous blood pressure variation: Hidden adaptability. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 143–169). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion. Psychological Review, 7, 516–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144–156. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg016

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassam, K. S., Markey, A. R., Cherkassky, V. L., Loewenstein, G., & Just, M. A. (2013). Identifying emotions on the basis of neural activation. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e66032. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lampl, M., Mummert, A., & Schoen, M. (2016). The lived experience of growing. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 47–66). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1994). The varieties of emotional experience: A meditation of James-Lange theory. Psychological Review, 101, 211–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lange, C. (1922). The emotions. (I. A. Haupt, Trans.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leshner, A. I. (1978). An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, L. S. (2016). Objective and subjective aspects of the drive to eat in obesogenic environments. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 195–230). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malina, R. M., Cumming, S. P., & Coelho e Silva, M. J. (2016). Physical activity and inactivity among children and adolescents: Assessment, trends and correlates. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 67–102 ). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Kremen, I. (1958). Autonomic feedback: A correlational study. Journal of Personality, 26, 388–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., Mandler, J. M., Kremen, I., & Sholiton, R. D. (1961). The response threat: Relations among verbal and physiological indices. Psychological Monographs, 75(2), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., Mandler, J. M., & Uviller, E. T. (1958). Autonomic feedback: The perception of autonomic activity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 56, 367–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre, M. H., & Hooven, C. K. (2009). Human sex differences in social relationships: Organizational and activational effects of androgens. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of Social Relationships (pp. 225–245). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagai, Y. (2015). Modulation of autonomic activity in neurological conditions: Epilepsy and Tourette Syndrome. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 278. doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00278

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., & Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 10127–10134. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2161-10.2010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, I. L. (2016). Calibrating the next generation: Mothers, early life experiences, and reproductive development. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 13–27). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prasad, A., Mumford, S. L., Buck, L. G. M., Ahrens, K. A., Sjaarda, L. A., Schliep, K. C., et al. (2014). Sexual activity, endogenous reproductive hormones and ovulation in premenopausal women. Hormones and Behavior, 66(2), 330–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Primus, R. J., & Kellogg, C. K. (1989). Pubertal-related changes influence the development of environment-related social interaction in the male rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 633–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudzik, A. E. F., & Ball, H. L. (2016). Baby-lag: Methods for assessing parental tiredness and fatigue. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 29–46). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, R., Parkin, J. C., Chen, J. Y., & Gray, P. B. (2009). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of Social Relationships (pp. 319–339). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399. doi:10.1037/h0046234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schandry, R., Bestler, M., & Montoya, P. (1993). On the relation between cardiodynamics and heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology, 30, 467–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sievert, L. L., Reza, A., Mills, P., Morrison, L. A., Rahberg, N. A., Goodloe, A., et al. (2010). Diurnal rhythm and concordance between objective and subjective hot flashes: The Hilo Women’s Health Study. Menopause, 17, 471–479. doi:10.1097/gme.0b013e3181cbb3c6

    Google Scholar 

  • Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26, 163–174. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Södersten, P., Damass, A., & Smith, E. R. (1977). Sexual behavior in developing male rats. Hormones and Behavior, 8, 320–341. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(77)90006-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 69–74. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, C. A., & Crane, T. A. (2016). Biomarkers of diet and nutritional health. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 171–193). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C.-W., & Kross, E. (2013). An fMRI-Based Neurologic Signature of Physical Pain. New England Journal of Medicine, 368, 1388–1397. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1204471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. T., & Maki, P. M. (2016). Subjective memory complaints and objective memory performance. In L. L. Sievert & D. E. Brown (Eds.), Biological measures of human experience across the lifespan: Making visible the invisible (pp. 275–299). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalan, R. E. (1967). Hormones and behavior. In R. E. Whalen (Ed.), Hormones and behavior: An enduring problem in psychology (pp. 3–20). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, A. J., Day Baird, D., Dunson, D. B., McConnaughey, D. R., Kesner, J. S., & Weinberg, C. R. (2004). On the frequency of intercourse around ovulation: Evidence for biological influences. Human Reproduction, 19, 1539–1543. doi:10.1093/humrep/deh305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1897). Grundriss der Psychologie, von Wilhelm Wundt. Leipzig: W. Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel E. Brown .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brown, D.E., Sievert, L.L. (2016). Making Visible the Invisible. In: Sievert, L., Brown, D. (eds) Biological Measures of Human Experience across the Lifespan. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-44103-0_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-44103-0_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-44101-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-44103-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics