Skip to main content

Emotional Relativity—Argument from Nurture

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Emotional Prosody Processing for Non-Native English Speakers

Part of the book series: The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series ((BMBBS,volume 3))

  • 606 Accesses

Abstract

The anthropological principle of linguistic relativity constitutes the framework of anthropological research on all aspects of communication including emotions. Treated as an observable and immutable principle in anthropology, linguistic relativity has long been largely misconstrued outside its original discipline. Through a process of vulgarization, that is misinterpretation through oversimplification and decontextualization (Joseph 2002), the principle became known in other disciplines as the Sapir–Whorf Hypothesis. This chapter describes the development of the original linguistic relativity principle from the works of Franz Boas to the major contributions from Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf. It then traces the roots of the process of vulgarization of the principle which transformed it into the long controversial Sapir–Whorf Hypothesis. Recent reconciliatory developments in emotion psychology towards the original formulation of linguistic relativity principle are also included. Following this historical analysis, the anthropological literature on the intersection of language and emotions is reviewed in sections devoted to emotion–specific syntax, semantics, and pragmatics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Margaret Mead’s Coming of age in Samoa (1928) was a widely read and appreciated book in both popular and academic circles. The book made her name but over time garnered controversy for both its candid descriptions of sexual practices in the Samoan community and the lack of professional distance an impartiality in her descriptions of Samoan cultural practices.

  2. 2.

    Ferdinand de Saussure—a Swiss scholar mostly honored for his major contributions to linguistics and semiotics. The basic tenet of Saussurean theory of language was that it has two principal forms. The first was la langue, that is the abstract, inherent, and perfect system of meanings and grammar retained in the mind. The second was la parole, that is the actual manifestation of la langue in speech and interpersonal communication, which was imperfect but the only means of accessing and analyzing la langue (Saussure 1959).

  3. 3.

    Even Nietzshe was not quite so radical. David Lovekin traced the origins of the “prison-house” metaphor to a poetic mistranslation of Nietzshe’s words by Erich Heller. The original wording from Nietzshe implied that the logical thought is “constrained” rather than “imprisoned” by language (Lovekin 1991).

  4. 4.

    Bronislaw Malinowski—a Polish anthropologist and ethnographer most famous for his works on the Trobriand Island cultures, largely covered by his seminal works Argonauts of the Western Pacific: An account of native enterprise and adventure in the Archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea (1922) and The Sexual Life of Savages in North-Western Melanesia (1929). His greatest methodological contribution to anthropology ws the participant observation method.

  5. 5.

    Romaji—a style of notation of the Japanese language using the Latin alphabet.

  6. 6.

    Coining the term “emotion” to denote a coherent category of psychosomatic experiences is usually attributed to Descartes who first described it in his 1649 essay Passions de l’Ame (Frijda 2008).

  7. 7.

    The Great Emigration—a significant and heavily romanticized episode of Polish cultural history typically defined as the period just before, during and immediately following the gradual partition of the country between Austria, Prussia, and Russia in the 19th century. The Great Emigration was an exodus of the intellectual elite from Poland to continue their political, scientific, and creative work in countries sympathetic to the Polish cause. Frédéric Chopin was one of those Great Emigrants.

References

  • Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, Lisa Feldman. (2011). Constructing emotion. Psychological Topics, 20(3), 359–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besnier, N. (1986). Word order in Tuvaluan. In P. Geraghty, L. Carrington, & S. A. Wurm (Eds.), FOCAL I: Papers from the fourth international conference on Austronesian linguistics (pp. 245–268). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1910). Psychological problems in anthropology. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 371–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1911). Introduction. In F. Boas (Ed.), Handbook of American Indian languages (pp. 5–83). Washington: G.P.O.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1938). Language. In Franz Boas (Ed.), General anthropology (pp. 124–145). Boston: D.C. Heath and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briggs, Jean L. (1970). Never in anger. Portrait of an Eskimo family. Boston: Aldine Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. W. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55(1), 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. W., & Lenneberg, E. H. (1954). A study in language and cognition. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49(3), 454–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, Roger. (1958). Words and things. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1956). Introduction. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 1–34). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, B. (1976). Ergativity. In W. P. Lehmann (Ed.), Syntactic typology: Studies in the phenomenology of language (pp. 329–393). Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, Charles. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. New York: D. Appleton and Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2001). Linguistic anthropology: History, ideas, and issues. In Alessandro Duranti (Ed.), Linguistic anthropology: A reader (pp. 1–38). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2003). Language as culture in U.S. anthropology. Current Anthropology, 44(3), 323–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3/4), 169–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fajans, J. (1983). Shame, social action, and the person among the Baining. Ethos, 11(3), 166–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (2008). The psychologist’s point of view. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 68–88). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendron, M., Roberson, D., van der Vyver J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Cultural relativity in perceiving emotion from vocalizations. Psychological Science, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goddard C., & Wierzbicka A. (2008). Universal Human Concepts as a Basis for Contrastive Linguistic Semantics. In M. A. G. Gonzales, J. Lachlan Mackenzie & E. M. Gonzales Alvarez (Eds.). Current Trends in Contrastive Linguistics: Functional and cognitive perspectives (pp. 205–226). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. New York: Anchor Books Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayakawa, S. I. (1947). Language in action. A guide to accurate thinking, reading, and writing. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janney, R. W. (1996). Speech and affect: Emotive uses of English. Unpublished manuscript. Munich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, John. (2002). From Whitney to Chomsky. Essays in the history of American linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, M. S. (1979). Non-verbal communication across cultures. The Modern Language Journal, 63(8), 416–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinsmith, A., De Silva, P. R., & Bianchi-Berthouse, N. (2006). Cross-cultural differences in recognizing affect from body posture. Interacting with Computers, 18, 1371–1389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koerner, E. F. K. (2003). Toward a history of American linguistics. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labarre, W. (1947). The cultural basis of emotions and gestures. Journal of Personality, 16(1), 49–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laukka, P., Neiberg, D., & Elfenbein, H. A. (2014). Evidence for cultural dialects in vocal emotion expression: Acoustic classification within and across five nations. Emotion, 14(3), 445–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, J. (1996). Meaning and feeling in the anthropology of emotions. American Ethnologist, 23(3), 514–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann, H. (2009). Autonomy, beneficence, and gezelligheit: Lessons in moral theory from the Dutch. Hastings Center Report, 39(5), 39–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linnett, J. T. (2011). Money can’t buy me hygge. Danish middle-class consumption, egalitarianism, and the sanctity of inner space. Social Analysis, 55(2), 21–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovekin, D. (1991). Technique, discourse, and consciousness: An introduction to the philosophy of Jacques Ellul. London and Toronto: Associated University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, C. (1982). The domain of emotion words on Ifaluk. American Ethnologist, 9(1), 113–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, C., & White, G. M. (1986). Anthropology of emotions. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15, 405–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1922). Argonauts of the Western Pacific: An account of native enterprise and adventure in the Archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B., & Ellis H. (1929). The sexual life of savages in North-Western Melanesia. An ethnographic account of courtship, marriage, and family life among the natives of the Trobriand Islands, British New Guinea. New York: Eugenics Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa. A psychological Study of Primitive Youth for Western Civilization. New York: William Morrow & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, C. M., Difendorff, J. M., & Greguras, J. G. (2012). Understanding emotional display rules at work and outside of work: The effects of country and gender. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 323–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niiya, Y., Ellsworth, P. C., & Yamaguchi, S. (2006). Amae in Japan and the United States: An exploration of a ‘culturally unique’ emotion. Emotion, 6(2), 279–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niyekawa-Howard, A. M. (1968). A psycholinguistic study of the Whorfian hypothesis based on the Japanese passive. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nöth, W. (1995). Handbook of semiotics. Stuttgart: J. B. Metzlersche Verlagsbuchhandlung.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novin, S., Barenjee, R., Dadkhah, A., & Rieffe, C. (2008). Self-reported use of emotional display rules in the Netherlands and Iran: evidence for sociocultural influence. Review of Social Development, 18(2), 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2005). Bilingualism and thought. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism. Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 433–453). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2013). Sapir, Whorf and the hypothesis that wasn’t. http://cup.linguistlist.org/academic-books/sapir-whorf-and-the-hypothesis-that-wasnt. Accessed: 20 July 2014.

  • Pesetsky, D. (1995). Zero syntax: Experiencers and cascades. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosaldo, M. (1980). Knowledge and passion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of Categorication. In E. Margolis and S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: Core Readings (pp. 189–206). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safdar, S., Friedlmeier, W., Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Kwantes, C. T., Kakai, H., et al. (2009). Variations of emotional display rules within and across cultures: A comparison between Canada, USA, and Japan. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 41(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1921). Language. An introduction to the study of speech. (Kindle Ed.). The Project Gutenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1924). The grammarian and his language. American Mercury, 1, 149–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1926). Notes on psychological orientation in a given society: Hanover conference presentation and excerpts of discussion. (Reprinted from R. Darnell & J. T. Irvine (Eds.), (1999), The collected works of Edward Sapir III: Culture (pp. 73–97). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1927–32). The psychology of culture, notes from lectures by Edward Sapir made by David Mandelbaum. (Reprinted from R. Darnell & J. T. Irvine (Eds.), (1999), The collected works of Edward Sapir III: Culture (pp. 421–675). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1927a). Speech as a personality trait. American Journal of Sociology 32, 892–905. (Reprinted from R. Darnell & J. T. Irvine (Eds.), (1999), The collected works of Edward Sapir III: Culture (pp. 120-132). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1927b). Language as a form of human behavior. The English Journal 16, 421–433. (Reprinted from R. Darnell, & J. T. Irvine (Eds.), (1999), The collected works of Edward Sapir III: Culture (pp. 204-216). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1930). The cultural approach to the study of personality: Hanover conference presentation and excerpts of discussion. (Reprinted from R. Darnell & J. T. Irvine (Eds.), (1999), The collected works of Edward Sapir III: Culture (pp. 199–254). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York: Philosophical Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semnani-Azad, Z., & Adair, W. L. (2011). The display of ‘dominant’ nonverbal cues in negotiation: The role of culture and gender. International Negotiation, 11, 452–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solzhenitsyn, A. (1973). Arkhipelag Gulag. Paris: YMCA Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Song, L., Leavell, A. S., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Yoshikawa, H. (2012). Ethnic differences in mother-infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants. Developmental Science, 15(3), 384–397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1938). Language: Plan and conception of arrangement, personal memo circulated by Whorf in his department in 1938. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 125–133). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1939a). The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 134–159). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1939b). Gestalt technique of stem composition in Shawnee. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 160–172). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1940a). Science and linguistics. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 207–219). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1940b). Linguistics as an exact science. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 220–232). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1941a). Languages and logic. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 233–245). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Semantics, culture, and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on who is watching. Child Development, 67, 957–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Halszka Bąk .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bąk, H. (2016). Emotional Relativity—Argument from Nurture. In: Emotional Prosody Processing for Non-Native English Speakers. The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series, vol 3. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-44042-2_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics