Skip to main content

The Situatedness of Pragmatic Acts: Explaining a Lamp to a Robot

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Pragmemes and Theories of Language Use

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology ((PEPRPHPS,volume 9))

  • 875 Accesses

Abstract

That human pragmatic action is heavily dependent on, and interacts with, the situation of utterance, is illustrated by Jacob Mey (J Pragmat 42:2882–2888, 2010) by contrasting human conversation with interactions with artificial intelligence, where the utterances produced are characterized by a lack of situatedness and are thus excellent examples of speech acts that are not pragmatic acts. In the current paper, I also use interactions with artificial communication partners to investigate the effects of different degrees of situatedness on pragmatic actions. In particular, I present an experiment in which people explain the use of an ordinary household item, a table-top lamp, either to their 10-month old children, or to a robot which resembles a young child. The robot’s behavior differs with respect to the degree to which the robot takes the joint interaction situation into account. The qualitative and quantitative analysis of tutors’ utterances shows that the communication partner, but also the degree of the robot’s situatedness, have a considerable impact on people’s verbal behavior. At the same time, tutors also bring in certain preconceptions about human-robot situations. Together, the results support a situated notion of the pragmatic act, the pragmeme.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Three participants’ data were not taken into account, as they only contained attention getting devices and non-verbal demonstrations.

References

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (1992). What computers still can’t do. A critique of artificial reason. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipi, A. (2009). Toddler and parent interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J. (1982). Frame semantics. In Linguistics in the morning calm (pp. 111–137). Seoul: Hanshin Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J. (1985). Frames and the semantics of understanding. Quadernidi Semantica, 6, 222–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J., & Atkins, B. T. (1992). Toward a frame-based lexicon: The semantics of risk and its neighbors. In A. Lehrer & E. F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields and contrasts (pp. 75–102). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K., Foth, K., Rohlfing, K., & Wrede, B. (2011). Mindful tutors – linguistic choice and action demonstration in speech to infants and to a simulated robot. Interaction Studies, 12(1), 134–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K., Lohan, K. S, Rohlfing, K., & Foth, K. (2014). Partner orientation in asymmetric communication: Evidence from contingent robot response. HRI ’14 workshop on humans and robots in asymmetric interactions, 3rd March 2014, Bielefeld, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. H., & Betancourt, E. A. (2006). Attention to maternal multimodal naming by 6- to 8-month-old infants and learning of word–object relations. Infancy, 9, 259–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2006). On the human ‘interaction engine’. In N. C. Enfield & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohan, K. S. (2011). A model of contingency detection to spot tutoring behavior and to respond to ostensive cues in human-robot interaction. PhD Thesis, Bielefeld University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohan, K. S., Rohlfing, K. J., Pitsch, K., Saunders, J., Lehmann, H., Nehaniv, C. L., Fischer, K., & Wrede, B. (2012). Tutor spotter: Proposing a feature set and evaluating it in a robotic system. International Journal of Social Robotics, 4(2), 131–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metta, G., Natale, L., Nori, F., Sandini, G., Vernon, D., Fadiga, L., von Hofsten, C., Rosander, K., Lopes, M., Santos-Victor, J., Bernardino, A., & Montesano, L. (2010). The iCub humanoid robot: An open-systems platform for research in cognitive development. Neural Networks, 23(8), 1125–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mey, J. L. (2001). Pragmatics: An introduction (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mey, J. L. (2010). Reference and the pragmeme. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 2882–2888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1988). The design of everyday things. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nückles, M., Wittwer, J., & Renkl, A. (2006). How to make instructional explanations in human tutoring more effective. In Proceedings of the 28th annual conference of the cognitive science society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohlfing, K. J., Fritsch, J., Wrede, B., & Jungmann, T. (2006). How can multimodal cues from child-directed interaction reduce learning complexity in robots? Advanced Robotics, 20(10), 1183–1199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1984). Minds, brains, and science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kerstin Fischer .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fischer, K. (2016). The Situatedness of Pragmatic Acts: Explaining a Lamp to a Robot. In: Allan, K., Capone, A., Kecskes, I. (eds) Pragmemes and Theories of Language Use. Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 9. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43491-9_44

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43491-9_44

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-43490-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-43491-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics