Skip to main content

Beyond the “Variables”: Developing Metalanguage for Psychology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Centrality of History for Theory Construction in Psychology

Part of the book series: Annals of Theoretical Psychology ((AOTP,volume 14))

Abstract

Over the twentieth century, psychology has adopted the scheme of causal thinking that involves the S → R (stimulus → response) basic structure. It brings into the thinking of psychologists the axiomatic acceptance of linear causality (“S causes R”) without a focus on elaboration of how the supposed process of causing actually operates. In the experimental and quasi-experimental practices of research, that scheme has become contextualized as the practice of specifying “independent” (manipulable) and “dependent” (outcome) factors called “variables,” creating the illusion of the researcher’s control over the processes under investigation in a context of an experiment (or its derivatives, such as questionnaires or interviews). This is unrealistic in the case of human psychological processes that are of the character of open systems characterized not by “effects” but by exchange relations with the environment (exemplified by Dewey in his replacement the “reflex arc” by the “reflex circle”) which operate on the basis of cyclical (catalyzed) rather than linear causality. The result is a situation—well captured by Ludwig Wittgenstein—that in psychology the problems and methods pass each other by. We trace the history of the terminology of “independent” variable as it became used in psychology, discuss the philosophical underpinnings of the notion of “variable” in a universe of dynamically structured and normatively guided psychological phenomena, and suggest that the notion of “variables” be abandoned and replaced by other concepts that would capture the qualitative nature of the human phenomena more adequately.

Psychology should rely on qualitative studies that go beyond observation of quantitative relationships between variables; studies should take into account that the phenomenon under study, mind, only manifests in behavior; qualitative levels of analysis should be clearly distinguished, it should be taken into account that elements in a whole cannot be independent in principle; studies should focus on cases instead of groups; typology is the main methodological tool for generalization in psychology; prediction without insight, without substantive theory, should not be acceptable, selection of facts should be systematically guided by theory; and scholars should not constrain themselves to interpretation of data provided by convenient methodological tools, such as statistical data analysis—psychologist should be more interested in thinking about the meaning of collected facts than in the accumulation of facts as such.

Aaro Toomela ( 2008 ), p. 262

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In researchers’ vernacular, “dependent variable”—some characteristic that varies as a result of changed experimental conditions.

  2. 2.

    The “independent variable”—a characteristic that the researcher changes at the input, or pretends to vary (e.g., gender as “independent variable”) and that is assumed to lead to changes in some outcome (“dependent variable”).

  3. 3.

    The “intervening variable”—something that “mediates” the “independent” and “dependent” variable.

  4. 4.

    A male clinical counselor told the story, decades after it happened, of interviewing an adolescent girl who, in response to the politically correct intervention to inquire if she has already entered the world of sexual relations (“Are you sexually active?”) in full honesty and openness, responded, “No, I just lay there.”

References

  • Branco, A. U., & Valsiner, J. (1997). Changing methodologies: A co-constructivist study of goal orientations in social interactions. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9(1), 35–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2011). Towards an expansive hybrid psychology: Integrating theories of the mediated mind. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45(1), 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2014). Psychiatric diagnoses as semiotic mediators: The case of ADHD. Nordic Psychology, 66(2), 121–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2016). Psychology as a normative science. In J. Valsiner et al. (Eds.), Psychology as the science of human being: The Yokohama Manifesto (Advances of theoretical psychology, Vol. 13, pp. 3–16). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S., Jacobsen, M. H., & Kristiansen, S. (2014). Historical overview of qualitative research in the social sciences. In P. Leavy (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of qualitative research (pp. 17–42). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabell, K. R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2014). The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality. In Annals of theoretical psychology (Vol. 11). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1997). Metalanguage: The technological framework. In K. Danziger (Ed.), Naming the mind: How psychology found its language (pp. 158–180). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3(3), 357–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1993). The superego, the ego, and the id in statistical reasoning. In G. Keren & C. Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: Methodological issues (pp. 311–339). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1996). From tools to theories: Discovery of cognitive psychology. In C. F. Graumann & K. J. Gergen (Eds.), Historical dimensions of psychological discourse (pp. 36–59). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Sturm, T. (2007). Tools = theories = data? On some circular dynamics in cognitive science. In M. G. Ash & T. Sturm (Eds.), Psychology’s territories: Historical and contemporary perspectives from different disciplines (pp. 305–342). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaveanu, V.-P. (2014). Distributed creativity. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R., & Moghaddam, F. (2012). Psychoneurology: The program. In R. Harré & F. Moghaddam (Eds.), Psychology for the third millennium: Integrating cultural and neuroscience perspectives (pp. 2–21). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. (2001). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture and Psychology, 7(3), 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H., & Gieser, T. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of dialogical self. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M., & Kempen, H. (1993). The dialogical self: Meaning as movement. San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurme, H. (1997). Psychological concepts, their producers and consumers. Culture and Psychology, 3(2), 115–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (2000). When things strike back—A possible contribution of “science studies” to the social sciences. British Journal of Sociology, 51, 107–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. (1985). How psychology makes itself true—Or false. In S. Koch & D. E. Leary (Eds.), A century of psychology as science (pp. 897–920). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1963). The second cybernetics: Deviation amplifying mutual causal processes. American Scientist, 51, 164–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1995). Individual epistemological heterogeneity across cultures and its use in organizations. Cybernetica, 37(3), 215–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mounin, G. (1981). A semiology of the sign system: Chemistry. Diogenes, 29, 216–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. N. (2001). Sigmund Koch—Philosophically speaking. American Psychologist, 56, 420–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & Much, N. (1987). Determinations of meaning: Discourse and moral socialization. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Moral development through social interaction (pp. 197–244). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegfried, J. (Ed.). (1994). The status of common sense in psychology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanggaard, L. (2014). Fooling around: Creative learning pathways. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008). Variables in psychology: A critique of quantitative psychology. IPBS: Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42, 245–265. doi:10.0007s12124-008-9059-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2010). Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2009). Baldwin’s quest: A universal logic of development. In J. W. Clegg (Ed.), The observation of human systems: Lessons from the history of anti-reductionistic empirical psychology (pp. 45–82). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2012). The guided science. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2015). The place for synthesis: Vygotsky’s analysis of affective generalization. History of the Human Sciences, 28(2), 93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, B. (2017). Frederic Bartlett. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This chapter was prepared with the support of Danske Grundforskningsfond through the Niels Bohr Professorship (J.V.) and a Sapere Aude study (Diagnostic Culture) funded by the Danish Council for Independent Research. Grant No. 12-125597 (S.B.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jaan Valsiner .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Valsiner, J., Brinkmann, S. (2016). Beyond the “Variables”: Developing Metalanguage for Psychology. In: Klempe, S., Smith, R. (eds) Centrality of History for Theory Construction in Psychology . Annals of Theoretical Psychology, vol 14. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-42760-7_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics