Skip to main content

Extending Cognition Through Superstition: A Niche-Construction Theory Approach

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Model-Based Reasoning in Science and Technology

Part of the book series: Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics ((SAPERE,volume 27))

  • 677 Accesses

Abstract

Superstitious practices have been considered since the ancient times as signs of deviating cognitive forms (such as the elders’), concerned with irrelevant causal relationships, and/or reducible to religious beliefs (and hence explained away). Recent theories such as the extended mind and cognitive niche construction, though, can shed new light on superstition and its apparently unreasonable success. The trigger is to observe how most superstitions are not mere “beliefs” (such as religious beliefs could be) hosted in a naked mind, but rather involve a strong coupling between the mind and some external props allowing its extensions away from the skull: from bodily gestures, to artifacts and other agents (human and animal). The mind’s capability to extend into the environment supports the related theory of cognitive niche construction, suggesting that human agents achieved better and better performances by creating external structures (cognitive niches) able to provide better and persistent scaffoldings for their cognitive performances. When it is not possible to detect and exploit the presence of a cognitive niche in the environment, superstitious practices can be identified as the possibility to deploy an emergency-cognitive niche projected by the superstitious agent into the world by means of a superstitious prop (item, ritual, gesture). It is poorer and less reliable but preferable to utter blank (and the consequent inaction), and most important it is still coupled with the external world (be it the body or its ecology in forms of artifacts and other agents), thus maintaining the fundamental characteristic of cognitive niches, that is distribution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Many entries are quite accurate, and their etymology is indeed illuminating as for the knowledge of the concept, while some other entries are outright hilarious.

  2. 2.

    Religious authorities and scholars have always fought a theological and intellectual battle against superstition, as interestingly shown by the dedicated entry in the Routledge Dictionary of Religious and Spiritual Quotations (Parrinder 2000, p. 24).

  3. 3.

    In agreement with (Odling-Smee et al. 2003, pp. 256–257), the concept of fitness here has to be intended as loosely Darwinian because of the following reasons: extragenetically informed behavior patterns are broadly adaptive and maladaptive; variants occurring during genetic evolution are random, whereas those of extra-genetic information are not.

  4. 4.

    I explicitly refer to how religion is received: this is not the lieu for an analysis of theological debates such as those relating to God’s grace, the relationship between faith and works, and obviously predestination. It is worth noticing, though, that also in theologies preaching predestination the fact that salvation is pre-established automatically excludes it from the kind of luck that can be affected by superstitious practices.

  5. 5.

    This can be linked to confabulatory behavior (Hirstein 2009) towards the exploitation of cognitive niches, inasmuch as an agent is unaware of her own projection—especially when superstition needs to discard a preexistent niche. Still, I uphold this consideration only to the extent that it does not involve a pathologization of superstition, which would utterly go against the spirit of this article.

  6. 6.

    This is akin to the difference between bullshitting (sic.) and lying in their relationship with truth (cf. Magnani 2011, Chap. 4.6; Frankfurt 2005). However noxious, the bullshitter’s lack of commitment towards the truth is usually mirrored by an equal lack of commitment towards the others’ commitment. A liar, instead, has to ensure that the others adhere to the commitment to truth she is denying.

References

  • Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bardone, E. (2011). Seeking chances: From biased rationality to distributed cognition. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barney, S. A., Lewis, W. J., Beach, J. A., & Berghof, O. (Eds.). (2006). The etymologies of Isidore of Seville. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. (2009). Cognitive science, religion and theology. In J. Schloss & M. J. Murray (Eds.), The believing primate (pp. 76–99). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J., & Forstmeier, W. (2007). Superstition and belief as inevitable by-products of an adaptive learning strategy. Human Nature, 18(1), 35–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertolotti, T., & Magnani, L. (2010). The role of agency detection in the invention of supernatural beings: An abductive approach. In L. Magnani, W. Carnielli, & C. Pizzi (Eds.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology. Abduction, logic, and computational discovery (pp. 195–213). Heidelberg/Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertolotti, T., & Magnani, L. (2015). Contemporary finance as a critical cognitive niche. Mind and Society, 14(2), 273–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. London: Vintage U.K. Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulbulia, J. (2009). Religiosity as mental time-travel. In J. Schloss & M. J. Murray (Eds.), The believing primate (pp. 44–75). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2005). Word, niche and super-niche: How language makes minds matter more. Theoria, 54, 255–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(8), 370–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damisch, L., Stoberock, B., & Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep your fingers crossed! How superstition improves performance. Psychological Science, 21(7), 1014–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, R. L., Laland, K., & Odling-Smee, F. J. (2003). Rethinking adaptation. The niche-construction perspective. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 46(1), 80–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (2006). Breaking the spell. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drees, W. B. (2010). Religion and science in context. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankfurt, H. (2005). On bullshit. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. E. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting and knowing. Hillsdale, JN: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirstein, W. (2009). Introduction. What is confabulation? In W. Hirstein (Ed.), Confabulation: Views from neuroscience, psychiatry, psychology and philosophy (pp. 1–12). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., & Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: A scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Phil Trans R Soc B, 367, 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D., & Bering, J. (2006). Hand of god, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 219–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Hoppitt, W. (2003). Do animals have culture? Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 150–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L. (2007). Creating chances through niche construction. The role of affordances. In B. Apolloni (Ed.), Knowledge-based intelligent information and engineering systems: 11th international conference, KES 2007, Vietri sul Mare, Italy, September 12–14, 2007, Proceedings, Part II. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L. (2009). Abductive cognition: The epistemological and eco-cognitive dimensions of hypothetical reasoning. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L. (2011). Understanding violence. Morality, religion, and violence intertwined: A philosophical stance. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L., & Bertolotti, T. (2013). Selecting chance curation strategies: Is chance curation related to the richness of a cognitive niche? International Journal of Knowledge and System Science, 4(1), 50–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1948). In R. Redfield (Ed.), Magic, science and religion and other essays. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmor, J. (1956). Some observations on superstitions in contemporary life. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 26, 119–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (Ed.). (2010). The extended mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (1999). Handaxes and ice age carvings: Hard evidence for the evolution of consciousness. In A. R. Hameroff, A. W. Kaszniak, & D. J. Chalmers (Eds.), Toward a science of consciousness III. The third Tucson discussions and debates (pp. 281–296). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1983). Some observations on mental models. In D. Gentner & Stevens A. L. (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 7–14). New York and London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction. A neglected process in evolution. New York, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrinder, G. (2000). The Routledge dictionary of religious and spiritual quotations. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2003). Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche. In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 16–37). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sage, J. (2004). Truth-reliability and the evolution of human cognitive faculties. Philosophical Studies, 117, 95–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C. (2015). Ontogenesis, semiosis and the epigenetic dynamics of biocultural niche construction. Cognitive Development. Online first, doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.006

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). Superstition in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), Primate models of hominid behavior (pp. 183–237). Albany: Suny Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vyse, S. (2014). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral. Chicago and London: Chicago University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tommaso Bertolotti .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this paper

Cite this paper

Bertolotti, T. (2016). Extending Cognition Through Superstition: A Niche-Construction Theory Approach. In: Magnani, L., Casadio, C. (eds) Model-Based Reasoning in Science and Technology. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 27. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-38983-7_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics