Advertisement

Spirituality and Cognitive Neuroscience: A Partnership for Refining Maps of the Mind

  • B. Les Lancaster
Chapter

Abstract

Spiritual and mystical traditions share with the approach of cognitive neuroscience an interest in mapping the mind. These traditions have always sought to find ways to promote a deeper understanding of our inner lives than simple untrained introspection on mental processes can reveal. Their insights arise through complex meditative, contemplative, and ritual practices directed at profoundly transforming the normal state of consciousness into one imbued with wisdom. These practices are complementary to the approach of cognitive neuroscience which studies states of mind from the outside, as it were. In this sense, an increasing number of neuroscientists view spirituality as a source of insights and practices that can be researched through neuroscientific analysis. There has been an exponential rise in research over some 20 years into changes in the brain and cognitive and emotional functions associated with spiritual practices. In large measure, claims by the spiritual traditions about the short- and long-term effects of these practices have been confirmed. Further areas in which cognitive neuroscience has drawn from the insights in spiritual and mystical traditions have focused on the nature of self and related mental processes such as memory and perception. The core feature in definitions of spirituality concerns our connectedness with a larger sphere—be that conceptualised in terms of a divine or more generally as an integral wholeness in the natural world. Given most neuroscientists’ faith in the hegemony of scientific materialism, this feature of spirituality has been more challenging, with a predominance of reductive theories. Nevertheless, ways in which the functioning of the brain may be incorporated into that larger picture are beginning to emerge, and—it is argued here—are likely to become increasingly influential in the future.

Keywords

The sacred Cognitive neuroscience Scientific naturalism Meditation Attention Mindfulness Kabbalah Recurrent processing 

References

  1. Banerjee, R. (2008). Buddha and the bridging relations. In R. Banerjee & B. Chakrabarti (Eds.), Models of brain and mind: Physical, computational and psychological approaches (Progress in brain research, Vol. 168). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  2. Baruss, I. (2010). Beyond scientific materialism: Toward a transcendent theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17(7–8), 213–231.Google Scholar
  3. Batchelor, S. (1997). Buddhism without beliefs: A contemporary guide to awakening. New York: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
  4. Beauregard, M. (2014). The primordial psyche. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 21(7–8), 132–157.Google Scholar
  5. Beauregard, M., & O’Leary, D. (2007). The spiritual brain: A neuroscientist’s case for the existence of the soul. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  6. Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? A canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 19–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cahn, R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 180–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40(8), 1239–1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Collins, S. (1982). Selfless persons: Imagery and thought in Theravada Buddhism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cousins, L. S. (1981). The Patthana and the development of the Theravadin Abhidhamma. Journal of the Pali Text Society, 9, 22–46.Google Scholar
  11. Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(5), 204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deikman, A. J. (1966). Deautomatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry, 29, 324–338.Google Scholar
  14. Edwards, J., Peres, J., Monti, D. A., & Newberg, A. B. (2012). The neurobiological correlates of meditation and mindfulness. In A. Moreira-Almeida & F. S. Santos (Eds.), Exploring frontiers of the mind-brain relationship (pp. 97–112). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fingelkurts, A. A., & Fingelkurts, A. A. (2009). Is our brain hardwired to produce God, or is our brain hardwired to perceive God? A systematic review on the role of the brain in mediating religious experience. Cognitive Processing, 10(4), 293–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. George, L. K., Larson, D. B., Koenig, H. G., & McCullough, M. E. (2000). Spirituality and health: What we know, what we need to know. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1), 102–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage, 59(1), 750–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hellner-Eshed, M. (2009). A river flows from Eden: The language of mystical experience in the Zohar (N. Wolski, Trans.). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Hick, J. (1989). An interpretation of religion: Human responses to the transcendental. New Haven: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., Jr., McCullough, M. E., Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., & Zinbauer, B. J. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal of Theory of Social Behavior, 30, 51–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lamme, V. A. F. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention: A case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17(5–6), 861–872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lamme, V. A. F. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 494–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lancaster, B. L. (1991). Mind, brain and human potential: The quest for an understanding of self. Shaftesbury: Element.Google Scholar
  25. Lancaster, B. L. (2004). Approaches to consciousness: The marriage of science and mysticism. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  26. Lancaster, B. L. (2011a). The hard problem revisited: From cognitive neuroscience to Kabbalah and back again. In H. Walach, S. Schmidt, & W. B. Jonas (Eds.), Neuroscience, consciousness, and spirituality (pp. 229–251). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lancaster, B. L. (2011b). The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness, mysticism and psi. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 30, 11–22.Google Scholar
  28. Lancaster, B. L. (2013a). Neuroscience and the transpersonal. In H. Friedman & G. Hartelius (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook of transpersonal psychology (pp. 223–240). Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lancaster, B. L. (2013b). The path to higher consciousness in Kabbalah and allied traditions: An approach from neuroscience and psychology. In P. Sriramamurti, P. Prashant, & A. Mohan (Eds.), Spiritual consciousness (pp. 148–159). New Delhi: New Age Books.Google Scholar
  30. Lazar, S.W., Kerr, C.E., Wasserman, R.H., Gray, J.R., Greve, D.N., Treadway, M.T., et al. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport, 16, 1893–1897.Google Scholar
  31. Lindeman, M., Blomqvist, S., & Takada, M. (2012). Distinguishing spirituality from other constructs: Not a matter of well-being but of belief in supernatural spirits. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 200(2), 167–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182439719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lopez, D. S., Jr. (2008). Buddhism and science: A guide for the perplexed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., & Gaser, C. (2009). The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. NeuroImage, 45, 672–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Luders, E., Phillips, O. R., Clark, K., Kurth, F., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2012). Bridging the hemispheres in meditation: Thicker callosal regions and enhanced fractional anisotropy (FA) in long-term practitioners. NeuroImage, 61(1), 181–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 16369–16373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 499–551). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. MacDonald, D. A. (2011). Studying spirituality scientifically: Reflections, considerations, recommendations. Journal of Management Spirituality & Religion, 8(3), 195–210. doi: 10.1080/14766086.2011.599145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. MacDonald, D. A., Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2013). Meditation: Empirical research and future directions. In G. Hartelius & H. L. Friedman (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of transpersonal psychology (pp. 433–458). Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 8. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Matt, D. C. (Trans. and Ed.). (2004–2014). The Zohar: Pritzker Edition. 8 vols. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nasr, S. H. (1993). The need for a sacred science. Richmond: Curzon Press.Google Scholar
  44. Newberg, A., D’Aquili, E., & Rause, V. (2001). Why God won’t go away: Brain science and the biology of belief. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
  45. Paloutzian, R. F., & Park, C. L. (2005). Recent progress and core issues in the science of the psychology of religion and spirituality. In R. F. Paloutzian & C. L. Park (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 3–22). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  46. Pappas, J., & Friedman, H. (2007). Toward a conceptual clarification of the terms “religious”, “spiritual”, and “transpersonal” as psychological constructs. In J. Pappas, W. Smythe, & A. Baydala (Eds.), Cultural healing and belief systems (pp. 22–54). Calgary: Temeron Books.Google Scholar
  47. Paulson, S., Davidson, R., Jha, A., & Kabat‐Zinn, J. (2013). Becoming conscious: the science of mindfulness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1303(1), 87–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Ratnayaka, S. (1981). Metapsychology of the Abhidharma. The Journal of the International Association of Buddhist Studies, 4(2), 76–88.Google Scholar
  49. Rubia, K. (2009). The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychology, 82, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Samuel, G. (2014). Between Buddhism and science, between mind and body. Religions, 5, 560–579. doi: 10.3390/rel5030560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schwartz, J. M., Stapp, H. P., & Beauregard, M. (2004). Quantum physics in neuroscience and psychology: A neurophysical model of mind–brain interaction. The Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 281, 1781. http://www-physics.lbl.gov/$\sim$stapp/PTRS.pdf.Google Scholar
  52. Shokek, S. (2001). Kabbalah and the art of being. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  53. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., van Beek, M., Skewes, J., Bjarkam, C. R., Stubberup, M., Bertelsen, J., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Long-term meditation is associated with increased gray matter density in the brain stem. Wolters Kluwer Health, 20, 170–174.Google Scholar
  55. Walach, H. (2011). Neuroscience, consciousness, spirituality—questions, problems and potential solutions: An introductory essay. In H. Walach, S. Schmidt, & W. B. Jonas (Eds.), Neuroscience, consciousness, and spirituality (pp. 1–22). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Walach, H. (2015). Secular spirituality: The next step towards enlightenment. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 11, 42–58.Google Scholar
  58. Wolfson, E. (2005). Language, eros, being: Kabbalistic hermeneutics and poetic imagination. New York: Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Zohar. Edited by R. Margoliot. 6th edn. 3 vols. Jerusalem: Mosad ha-Rav Kook, 1978. (See also Matt, 2004–2014).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The Alef Trust, School of Natural Sciences and PsychologyLiverpool John Moores UniversityLiverpoolUK

Personalised recommendations