Skip to main content

Risk-Taking in Samango Monkeys in Relation to Humans at Two Sites in South Africa

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Ethnoprimatology

Abstract

A fundamental step in the management and conservation of wild species is advancing our understanding of how animals perceive and use their habitat. Spatial variation in risk either from natural predation or human disturbance generates a “landscape of fear” that can be measured and assessed using experimental patch approaches such as giving-up densities (GUDs). For primates inhabiting a matrix of human habitation, exotic plantations, and indigenous forest, it is possible to explore the “risk-disturbance hypothesis,” which posits that human risk parallels natural predation risk in its effects on animals’ habitat use and decision-making. Here we report on a combination of studies on arboreal samango monkeys (Cercopithecus albogularis spp.) and their risk-sensitive foraging at two sites subject to varying anthropogenic pressures. Using GUDs experiments, we documented pronounced effects of humans on monkeys’ patch use but the impact of humans did not simplistically follow predictions from the risk-disturbance hypothesis. At a site with limited human infrastructure, monkeys exploited food patches at typically high-risk strata (ground level) more intensively in the presence of researchers, whom they likely perceived as shields against terrestrial predators (leopards Panthera pardus, caracals Caracal caracal). Meanwhile, at a predator-poor site where monkeys regularly came into contact with human infrastructure and gardens (adjacent to indigenous forest), the experiments indicated that monkeys preferred to forage in indigenous forest given experimental patches in both forest and gardens, where they showed sensitivity to risk (likely from both humans and domestic dogs Canis lupus familiaris). However, once already in gardens, monkeys depleted patches to extents similar to inside indigenous forest. In this chapter, we also elaborate on monkeys’ risk-sensitive responses to negative, neutral, and rewarding objects associated with humans, and explore the impact of prior experience on risk-taking behavior in proximity to humans and their infrastructure. We conclude that the risk-disturbance hypothesis cannot be used as a blanket term to describe primates’ interactions with humans, as we show here how nuanced and flexible samango monkeys’ risk-taking responses are.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Atickem, A. (2013). Landscape genetics and behavioral ecology of mountain nyala (Tragelaphus buxtoni) in the Southern Highlands of Ethiopia. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atickem, A., Loe, L. E., & Stenseth, N. C. (2014). Individual heterogeneity in use of human shields by mountain nyala. Ethology, 120, 715–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beale, C. M. (2007). The behavioral ecology of disturbance responses. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 20, 111–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beale, C. M., & Monaghan, P. (2004). Behavioral responses to human disturbance: A matter of choice? Animal Behavior, 68, 1065–1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beamish, E. K. (2010). Causes and consequences of mortality and mutilation in the Cape Peninsula baboon population, South Africa. M.Sc. Thesis, Department of Zoology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J. (2007). Fear, human shields and the redistribution of prey and predators in protected areas. Biology Letters, 3, 620–623.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S. (1988). Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22, 37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, K. L., Lawes, M. J., & Macleod, M. M. (1998). Evaluation of non-lethal control methods on problematic samango monkeys in the Cape Vidal Recreation Reserve, Greater St. Lucia Wetland Park. South African Journal of Wildlife Research, 28, 89–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase Grey, J., Kent, V. T., & Hill, R. A. (2013). Evidence of a high density population of harvested leopards in a montane environment. PloS One, 8, e82832.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Wrangham, R. W. (1987). Predation. In B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate Societies (pp. 227–239). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, B. T., & Hill, R. A. (2014). Living in a landscape of fear: The impact of predation, resource availability and habitat structure on primate range use. Animal Behavior, 88, 165–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creel, S., Fox, J. E., Hardy, A., Sands, J., Garrott, B., & Peterson, R. O. (2002). Snowmobile activity and glucocorticoid stress responses in wolves and elk. Conservation Biology, 16, 809–814. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.2002.00554.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, D. L., Linden, B., Wimberger, K., Nupen, L. J., Tordiffe, A. S. W., Taylor, P. J., et al. (2015). New insights into samango monkey speciation in South Africa. PloS One, 10(6), e0129988.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, S. E., & Brown, J. S. (2013). Identifying preferred habitat of samango monkeys (Cercopithecus (nictitans) mitis erythrarchus) through patch use. Behavioural Processes, 100, 214–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, S. E., Brown, J. S., & Linden, J. D. (2011). Identifying Sykes’ monkeys’, Cercopithecus albogularis erythrarchus, axes of fear through patch use. Animal Behavior, 81, 455–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frid, A., & Dill, L. M. (2002). Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conservation Ecology, 6, 11. Retrieved from http://www.consecol.org/vol6/iss1/art11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A., & Young, T. P. (1993). Human presence reduces predation in a free-ranging vervet monkey population in Kenya. Animal Behavior, 45, 1233–1235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones-Engel, L., Engel, G. A., Heidrich, J., Chalise, M., Poudel, N., Viscidi, R., et al. (2006). Temple monkeys and health implications of commensalism, Kathmandu, Nepal. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 12(6), 900–906.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, B. (2013). In pursuit of a panacea: mitigating human-baboon conflict in the Cape Peninsula, South Africa. Ph.D. Thesis, Department of Zoology, University of Cape Town, Cape Town.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laundré, J. W., Hernandez, L., Ripple, W. J. (2010). The landscape of fear: Ecological implications of being afraid. The Open Ecology Journal, 3, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawes, M. J. (1990). The distribution of the samango monkey (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus Peters, 1852 and Cercopithecus mitis labiatus 1. Geoffroy, 1843) and forest history in southern Africa. Journal of Biogeography, 17, 669–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leighton, P. A., Horrocks, J. A., & Kramer, D. L. (2010). Conservation and the scarecrow effect: Can human activity benefit threatened species by displacing predators? Biological Conservation, 143, 2156–2163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S. L. (1998). Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator–prey interactions. Bioscience, 48, 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathibane, N. (2014). Neophilia in garden-dwelling samango monkeys. Undergraduate Honors Thesis, University of the Free State, Qwaqwa, Free State, South Africa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makin, D. F., Payne, H. F. P., Kerley, G. I. H., & Shrader, A. M. (2012). Foraging in a 3-D world: How does predation risk affect space use of vervet monkeys? Journal of Mammalogy, 93, 422–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, D., & Sanz, C. (2003). Naïve encounters with chimpanzees in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. International Journal of Primatology, 24(2), 369–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muhly, T. B., Semeniuk, C., Massolo, A., Hickman, L., & Musiani, M. (2011). Human activity helps prey win the predator–prey space race. PloS One, 6, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, K., Richards, S. A., le Roux, A., & Hill, R. A. (2016). Influence of live-capture on risk perceptions of habituated samango monkeys. Journal of Mammalogy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, K., Wimberger, K., Richards, S. A., Hill, R. A., & le Roux, A. (In press). Monkeys manage risk in a garden-forest matrix. International Journal of Primatology

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, K., le Roux, A., Richards, S. A., Scheijen, C. P. J., & Hill, R. A. (2014). Human observers impact habituated samango monkeys’ perceived landscapes of fear. Behavioral Ecology, 25(5), 1199–1204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papworth, S., Milner-Gulland, E. J., & Slocombe, K. (2013). Hunted woolly monkeys (Lagothrix poeppigii) show threat-sensitive responses to human presence. PloS One, 8(4), e62000.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/

  • Rödl, T., Berger, S., Romero, L. M., & Wikelski, M. (2007). Tameness and stress physiology in a predator-naïve island species confronted with novel predation threat. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 577–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogala, J. K., Hebblewhite, M., Whittington, J., White, C. A., Coleshill, J., & Musiani, M. (2011). Human activity differentially redistributes large mammals in the Canadian Rockies national parks. Ecology and Society, 16, 16–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soltis, J., King, L. E., Douglas-Hamilton, I., Vollrath, F., & Savage, A. (2014). African elephant alarm calls distinguish between threats from humans and bees. PloS One, 9, e89403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stankowich, T. (2008). Ungulate flight responses to human disturbance: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Conservation, 141, 2159–2173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swedell, L. (2011). African Papionins. Diversity of social organization and ecological flexibility. In C. J. Campbell, A. Fuentes, K. C. MacKinnon, S. K. Bearder, & R. M. Stumpf (Eds.), Primates in Perspective (2nd ed., pp. 241–277). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tadesse, S. A., & Kotler, B. P. (2012). Impact of tourism on Nubian ibex (Capra nubiana) revealed through assessment of behavioral indicators. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 1257–1262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, E. A. & Feistner, A. T. C. (2011). Habituating primates: processes, techniques, variables and ethics. In J.M. Setchell & D.J. Curtis (Eds.), Field and Laboratory Methods in Primatology: A Practical Guide (pp. 33–50). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willems, E. P., & Hill, R. A. (2009). Predator-specific landscapes of fear and resource distribution: Effects on spatial range use. Ecology, 90, 546–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimberger, K., Nowak, K., & Richards, S. A.Hill, R. A. (In review). Reliance on exotic plants by two groups of endangered samango monkeys at their southern range limit. International Journal of Primatology.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

K. Nowak was funded by a Durham University COFUND research fellowship, Claude Leon Foundation postdoctoral grant, and the R. W. Primate Fund during the course of this research. Fieldwork at Lajuma was supported by an Earthwatch grant to R. Hill. A. le Roux was supported, in part, by incentive funding for rated researchers by the National Research Foundation. K. Wimberger was supported by the Claude Leon Foundation, UCT URC Fellowship, JMasters NRF Fund, Novartis SAVF Wildlife Research Fund, Primate Conservation Inc., and Mazda Wildlife Fund during her research in Hogsback. We thank Ian Gaigher and the Primate and Predator Project (PPP) team in Lajuma, and Steve Boyes in Hogsback for logistical support and assistance in the field. We are especially grateful to Ciska Scheijen, Alison Midgley, Diana Breshears, Nthabiseng Mathibane, and PPP for assisting with GUDs experiments in the field. We also thank the community of Hogsback for allowing us to run experiments in their gardens. Shane Richards advised on models in R. Ethical approval came from Durham University’s Life Sciences Ethical Review Process Committee and from the University of the Free State’s Inter-Faculty Animal Ethics Committee. Research permission was granted to K. N. by the Department of Economic Development, Environmental Affairs and Tourism and by the Department of Water Affairs and Forestry.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katarzyna Nowak Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendix 1: Model 1

Linear mixed-effects model fit by REML

AIC BIC logLik

12701.97 12746.64 −6342.986

Random effects: Formula: ~ExpDay | Tree

Structure: General positive-definite, Log-Cholesky parametrization

StdDev Corr

(Intercept) 2.2702498 (Intr)

ExpDay 0.1373389 −0.652

Residual 5.9674331

Fixed effects: PeanutsLeft ~ BasinHeight + Researchers + BasinHeight:Researchers

 

Value

Std. error

DF

t-Value

p-Value

(Intercept)

 7.702045

0.4870477

1947

 15.813737

0.0000

BasinHeight

−0.637220

0.0599672

1947

−10.626151

0.0000

Researchers

−2.619970

0.4862516

1947

 −5.388094

0.0000

BasinHeight:Researchers

 0.1028469

1947

2.841911

0.0045

0.292282

Appendix 2: Model 2

glm (Visit ~ Location + Site + Group, family = “binomial”)

Deviance Residuals:

Min 1Q Median 3Q Max

−2.4478 0.3203 0.5323 0.7338 0.9669

Coefficients: (1 not defined because of singularities)

 

Estimate

Std. error

z Value

Pr (> |z|)

(Intercept)

 1.8827

0.2342

 8.038

9.14e−16***

LocationG

−1.3651

0.2862

−4.770

1.84e−06***

SiteLajuma

 1.0617

0.4318

 2.459

0.0139*

GroupHouse

−1.7698

0.4074

−4.344

1.40e−05***

Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘ ’ 1

(Dispersion parameter for binomial family taken to be 1)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nowak, K., Hill, R.A., Wimberger, K., le Roux, A. (2016). Risk-Taking in Samango Monkeys in Relation to Humans at Two Sites in South Africa. In: Waller, M. (eds) Ethnoprimatology. Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-30469-4_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics