Skip to main content

Children’s Natural Ways of Educating Themselves Still Work: Even for the Three Rs

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

Most educational research has been conducted in standard school settings. In this chapter, the author describes how children become educated in hunter-gatherer cultures, at a radically alternative school, and in families that have chosen to “unschool” their children—all of which are settings where children and adolescents are in charge of their own education. The author concludes that, given an optimal self-educational environment, the instinctive ways of learning that evolved to serve hunter-gatherers’ needs still function well in our culture today, such that coercive, top-down education is not necessary. The optimal environment for self-education includes (a) the expectation that education is children’s responsibility; (b) unlimited freedom to play, explore, and pursue one’s own interests; (c) opportunity to play with the tools of the culture; (d) access to a variety of caring adults, who are helpers, not judges; (e) free age mixing; and (f) immersion in a stable, moral, democratic community.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ashcraft, M. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benezet, L. (1936). The teaching of arithmetic: The story of an experiment. Journal of the National Education Association in three parts, 24(8), 241–244. 24(9), 301–303; 25(1), 7–8 (Original work published 1935).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. (2005). What makes a competent adult forager? In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives (pp. 109–128). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J., & Johnson, S. (2004). Subsistence ecology and play among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. Human Nature, 15, 63–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burns, M. (1998). Math: Facing an American phobia. Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlsson-Paige, N., Almon, J., & McLaughlin, G. (2015). Reading instruction in kindergarten: Little to gain and much to lose. Boston, MA: Defending the Early Years. Retrieved from https://www.sarahlawrence.edu/cdi/media/pdf/ReadinginKindergartenreport.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darling-Hammond, L., & Snyder, J. (1992). Curriculum studies and the traditions of inquiry: The scientific tradition. In P. W. Jackson (Ed.), Handbook of research on curriculum (pp. 41–78). New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draper, P. (1976). Social and economic constraints on child life among the !Kung. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors (pp. 199–217). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forester, A. (1977). What teachers can learn from “natural readers”. Reading Teacher, 31, 160–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky, E., Bond, J., & Friedman, D. (1993). The changing workforce: Highlights of the national study. New York, NY: Families and Work Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43, 179–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldbeck, S. (2001). Instructional models for early childhood. In S. Goldbeck (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on early childhood education. Reframing dilemmas in research and practice (pp. 3–31). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosso, Y., Otta, E., de Lima, M., Ribeiro, F., & Bussab, V. (2005). Play in hunter-gatherer societies. In A. D. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 213–253). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2009). Play as the foundation for hunter-gatherer social existence. American Journal of Play, 1, 476–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2010a). Kids learn math easily when they control their own learning. Freedom to Learn blog, Psychology Today. Retrieved April 15, 2010 from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn

  • Gray, P. (2010b). Unschoolers’ accounts of how children learn to read. Freedom to Learn blog, Psychology Today. Retrieved February 24, 2010 from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn

  • Gray, P. (2011a). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. American Journal of Play, 3, 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2011b). The special value of age-mixed play. American Journal of Play, 3, 500–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2013). Free to learn: Why releasing the instinct to play will make our children happier, more self-reliant, and better students for life. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2014). The play theory of hunter-gatherer egalitarianism. In D. Narvaez, K. Valentino, A. Fuentes, J. McKenna, & P. Gray (Eds.), Ancestral landscapes in human evolution: culture, childrearing and social wellbeing (pp. 190–213). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P., & Chanoff, D. (1986). Democratic schooling: What happens to young people who have charge of their own education? American Journal of Education, 94, 182–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P., & Feldman, J. (1997). Patterns of age mixing and gender mixing among children and adolescents at an ungraded democratic school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 67–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P., & Feldman, J. (2004). Playing in the zone of proximal development: Qualities of self- directed age mixing between adolescents and young children at a democratic school. American Journal of Education, 110, 108–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P., & Riley, G. (2013). The challenges and benefits of unschooling according to 232 families who have chosen that route. Journal of Unschooling and Alternative Learning, 7, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P., & Riley, G. (2015). Grown unschoolers’ evaluations of their unschooling experiences: Report I on a survey of 75 unschooled adults. Other Education, 4 (2), 8–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, D., & Sadofsky, M. (1992). Legacy of trust: Life after the Sudbury Valley School experience. Framingham, MA: Sudbury Valley School Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, D., Sadofsky, M., & Lempka, J. (2005). The pursuit of happiness: The lives of Sudbury Valley alumni. Framingham, MA: Sudbury Valley School Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B., Fouts, H., Boyette, A., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1168–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (1999). On the social relations of the hunter-gatherer band. In R. B. Lee & R. H. Daly (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (pp. 399–410). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L. G. (2015). Lively minds: Distinctions between academic versus intellectual goals for young children. Boston, MA: Defending the Early Years. Retrieved from https://deyproject.files.wordpress.com/2015/04/dey-lively-minds-4-8-15.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J. (2008). Research and the reading wars. In F. M. Hess (Ed.), When research matters: How scholarship influences education policy (pp. 89–111). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 285–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (1975). Relations among infants and juveniles in comparative perspective. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The origins of behavior (Friendship and peer relations, Vol. 4, pp. 99–129). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. (1988). Reflections on primitive communism. In T. Ingold, D. Riches, & J. Woodburn (Eds.), Hunters and gatherers (Vol. 1, pp. 252–268). Oxford, England: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R., & Daly, R. (1999). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemann, N. (1997). The reading wars. The Atlantic Monthly, 280(5), 128–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liebenberg, L. (1990). The art of tracking: The origin of science. Claremont, South Africa: Philip.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (2015). More play, please: The perspective of kindergarten teachers on play in the classroom. American Journal of Play, 7, 347–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcon, R. (2002). Moving up the grades: Relationship between preschool model and later school success. Early Childhood Research & Practice, 4, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margrain, V. (2005). Precocious readers: Case studies of spontaneous learning, self-regulation and social support in the early years. Doctoral dissertation, Victoria University of Wellington, School of Education, New Zealand

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, J. (1988). Absolutism and the eighteenth-century origins of compulsory schooling in Prussia and Austria. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newsom, C., Archer, R., Trumbetta, S., & Gottesman, I. (2003). Changes in adolescent response patterns on the MMPI/MMPI-A across four decades. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81, 74–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, L., Evans, J., & Keckler, W. (2006). Precocious readers: Past, present, and future. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30, 205–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley, G., & Gray, P. (2015). Grown unschoolers’ experiences with higher education and employment: Report II on a survey of 75 unschooled adults. Other Education, 4(2), 33–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweinhart, L., & Weikart, D. (1997). The high/scope preschool curriculum comparison study through age 23. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 117–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shostak, M. (1981). Nisa: The life and words of a !Kung woman. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sylva, K., Bruner, J., & Genova, P. (1976). The role of play in the problem solving of children 3–5 years old. In J. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play (pp. 244–257). New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, C. (1961). The forest people. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J., Gentile, B., DeWall, C., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., & Schurtz, D. (2010). Birth cohort increase in psychopathology among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 145–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J., Zhang, L., & Im, C. (2004). It’s beyond my control: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of increasing externality in locus of control, 1960–2002. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 308–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). In E. Hoffman & G. Vaker (Eds. & Trans.), Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind and society: The development of higher psychological processes (pp. 79–91). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wannenburgh, A. (1979). The bushmen. New York, NY: Mayflower Books.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Gray .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gray, P. (2016). Children’s Natural Ways of Educating Themselves Still Work: Even for the Three Rs. In: Geary, D., Berch, D. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics