Skip to main content

The Adaptive Value of Cognitive Immaturity: Applications of Evolutionary Developmental Psychology to Early Education

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

This chapter briefly outlines some of the principles of evolutionary developmental psychology, emphasizing that some aspects of young children’s cognitive immaturity are actually adaptations for surviving the niche of childhood and not simply deficits or shortcomings that need to be overcome. We take an evolutionary developmental perspective to describe how young children learn, focusing on learning through observation and play. We argue that educators can take advantage of some of these features of young children’s thinking and learning styles, and that some efforts to accelerate young children’s cognitive development, such as educational videos and direct-instruction preschools, can actually be counterproductive to early education.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In P. Mellers & C. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 455–513). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Academy of Pediatrics. (2011). Media use by children younger than 2 years. Pediatrics, 128, 1040–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development: Six theories of child development (Vol. 6). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L. S., Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2014). Less-structured time in children’s daily lives predicts self-directed executive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R. (2010). Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. Developmental Review, 30, 128–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 205–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Lauricella, A., Zack, E., & Calvert, S. L. (2010). Infant and early childhood exposure to adult-directed and child-directed television programming: Relations with cognitive skills at age four. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56, 21–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Muentener, P., Garcia, A., Fujimoto, M., & Chavez, V. (2007). Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. Developmental Science, 10, 910–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. P. G. (1976). Rules and reciprocity in behavioural development. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 401–421). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., Mann, T., & Ogan, A. (2006). Make-believe play: Wellspring for development of self-regulation. In D. G. Singer, R. M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Play = learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp. 74–100). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., & Meyers, A. B. (2013). The role of make-believe and the development of executive functions: Status of research and future directions. American Journal of Play, 6, 98–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). A note on neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 7, 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2007a). The most educable of species. In J. S. Carlson & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on contemporary educational issues (pp. 119–129). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2007b). Why youth is not wasted on the young. Malden, MA: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 13–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 34, 225–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Gaultney, J. F., & Green, B. L. (1993). “I watch, therefore I can do:” The development of meta-imitation over the preschool years and the advantage of optimism in one’s imitative skills. In R. Pasnak & M. L. Howe (Eds.), Emerging themes in cognitive development (Vol. 1, pp. 79–102). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Green, B. L. (1992). The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity. American Psychologist, 47, 46–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Hernández Blasi, C., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). Evolutionary developmental psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Evolutionary psychology handbook (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Periss, V., & Causey, K. (2009). The benefits of youth. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 120–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 899–911.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (1999). Patterns of human growth (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discover. Cognition, 120, 322–330.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boulton, M. J., & Smith, P. K. (1990). Affective bias in children’s perceptions of dominance relations. Child Development, 61, 221–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). Colloquium Paper: The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10918–10925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs (Revth ed.). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C. P., & Lan, Y. (2013). The influence of developmentally appropriate practice on children’s cognitive and psychosocial development: A qualitative metasynthesis. Teachers College Record, 115, 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., DeWolf, D. M., Ray, J., Manuel, K., & Fleege, P. O. (1993). Developmental appropriateness of kindergarten programs and academic outcomes in first grade. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 8, 23–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., & Kirk, L. (1990). A comparison of frequencies of stress behaviors observed in kindergarten children in classrooms with developmentally appropriate versus developmentally inappropriate instructional practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 407–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-olds. Child Development, 84, 422–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & White, R. E. (2013). Executive function, pretend play, and imagination. In M. Taylor (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination (pp. 161–174). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., White, R. E., & Davis-Unger, A. C. (2014). Evidence for a relationship between executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 29, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function, pretend play, and imagination. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, vol. 1: Body and mind (pp. 1–17). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cemore, J. J., & Herwig, J. E. (2005). Delay of gratification and make-believe play of preschoolers. Journal of Research in Early Childhood Education, 19, 251–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courage, M. L., Murphy, A. N., Goulding, S., & Setliff, A. E. (2010). When television is on: The impact of infant-directed video on 6- and 18-month-olds’ toy play and on parent-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 176–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courage, M. L., & Setliff, A. E. (2010). When babies watch television: Attention-getting, attention holding, and the implications for learning from video material. Developmental Review, 30, 220–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 366, 1149–1157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238, 1556–1557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., Chiong, C., Sherman, K., Islam, N., Vanderborght, M., Troseth, G. L.…O’Doherty, K. (2010). Do babies learn from baby media? Psychological Science, 21, 1570–1574.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., Pierroutsako, S. L., Uttal, D. H., Rosengren, K. S., & Gottlieb, A. (1998). Grasping the nature of pictures. Psychological Science, 9, 205–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 335–341.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). Brain and behaviour in primate evolution. In P. M. Kappler & J. B. Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 315–330). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Elias, C. L., & Berk, L. E. (2002). Self-regulation in young children: Is there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17, 216–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence?: Beyond the Flynn Effect. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froese, T., & Leavens, D. A. (2014). The direct perception hypothesis: Perceiving the intention of another’s action hinders its precise imitation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganea, P. A., Bloom Pickard, M., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9, 46–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, A., Greif, M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Guided by intention: Preschoolers’ imitation reflects inferences of causation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 31–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., & Perez, S. M. (2015). Cognitive development in cultural context. In L. Liben & U. Müller (Eds.) (R. M. Lerner, Gen Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 2: Cognitive processes (pp. 854–896). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 24–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2005). Folk knowledge and academic learning. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 493–519). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In J. S. Carlson & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Educating the evolved mind (Psychological perspectives on contemporary educational issues, Vol. 2, pp. 1–99). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71, 57–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6, 463–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Bluenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A.…Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilliam, W. S. (2005). Prekindergarteners left behind: Expulsion rates in state prekindergarten systems. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Child Study Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Lucas, C. G. (2015). When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 87–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Walker, C. M. (2013). Considering counterfactuals: The relationship between causal learning and pretend play. American Journal of Play, 6, 15–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. (2013). Free to learn: Why unleashing the instinct to play will make our children happier, more self-reliant, and better students for life. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M. (1998). The cultural evolution of IQ. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising cure: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 81–123). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greve, W., Thomsen, T., & Dehio, C. (2014). Does playing pay? The fitness-effect of free play during childhood. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 434–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Groos, K. (1898). The play of animals. New York, NY: Appleton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. (1959). The development of learning in the Rhesus monkey. American Scientist, 47, 459–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hernández Blasi, C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Berk, L. E., & Singer, D. G. (2009). A mandate for playful learning in preschool: Presenting the evidence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Hyson, M. C., & Rescolra, L. (1990). Academic environments in preschool: Challenge or pressure? Early Education and Development, 1, 401–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., Zettersten, M., Schleihauf, H., Grätz, S., & Pauen, S. (2014). The role of social interaction and pedagogical cues for eliciting and reducing overimitation in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 122–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofferth, S. L., & Sandberg, J. F. (2001). How American children spend their time. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 295–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo Sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, A. P., & Smith, P. K. (1987). Rough and tumble, friendship, and dominance in schoolchildren: Evidence for continuity and change with age. Child Development, 58, 201–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyson, M. C., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Rescorla, L. (1990). Academic environments in preschool: Challenge or pressure? Early Education and Development, 1, 401–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. B., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Brains, learning, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, P., & Turkewitz, G. (1986). Effects of unusually early visual stimulation on the development of homing behavior in the rat pup. Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 57–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R., Hammond, S., Dissanayake, C., & Ihsen, E. (2011). The relationship between symbolic play and executive function in young children. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 36(2), 21–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakocz, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraft, K. C., & Berk, L. E. (1998). Private speech in two preschools: Significance of open-ended activities and make-believe play for verbal self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 637–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. (2015). The anthropology of childhood (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D., & Grove, M. A. (2010). The role of adults in children’s learning. In D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood (pp. 145–179). New York, NY: AltaMira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R. (1990). Premature visual stimulation accelerates intersensory functioning in bobwhite quail neonates. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 15–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (1995). Intersensory experience and early perceptual development: Attenuated prenatal sensory stimulation affects postnatal auditory and visual responsiveness in bobwhite quail chicks (Colinus virginianus). Developmental Psychology, 31, 609–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, D. S., Drell, M. B., Richey, E., Boguszewski, K., & Smith, E. D. (2015). Further examination of the immediate impact of television on children’s executive function. Developmental Psychology, 51, 92–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. S., Li, H., & Boguszewski, K. (2015). Television and children’s executive function. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 48, 219–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. S., & Peterson, J. (2011). The immediate impact of different types of television on young children’s executive function. Pediatrics, 128, 644–649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linebarger, D. L., & Vaala, S. E. (2010). Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: An ecological perspective. Developmental Review, 30, 176–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., & Merriman, W. E. (2009). Persistent overconfidence despite practice: The roles of task experience in preschooler’s recall predications. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 152–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19751–19756.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McAuley, T., & White, D. A. (2011). A latent variables examination of processing speed, response inhibition, and working memory during typical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 45–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N. (2013). The influence of model status on the tendency of young children to over-imitate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 962–969.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, N., & Whiten, A. (2009). Emulation and “over-emulation” in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23-and 30-month-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 367–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Imitation of televised models by infants. Child Development, 59, 1221–1229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Crisis in the kindergarten: Why children need to play in school. College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Knowing the limits of one’s understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 1–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nathanson, A. I., Aladé, F., Sharp, M. L., Rasmusse, E. E., & Christy, K. (2014). The relation between television exposure and executive function among preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1497–1506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where culture takes hold: “Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85, 2169–2184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In K. J. Connolly & H. F. R. Prechtl (Eds.), Maturation and development: Biological and psychological perspectives (pp. 73–108). Philadelphia, PA: International Medical Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2007). Can play diminish ADHD and facilitate the construction of the social brain? Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 57–66.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, H. (1977). The development of learning ability in infancy (Entwicklung der Lernfähigkeit im Säuglingsalter). In G. Nissen (Ed.), Intelligence, learning, and learning disabilities (Intelligenz, Lernen und Lernstörungen). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D. (2013). Play. In P. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 276–299). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The ontogeny and phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Human Nature, 15, 23–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (Eds.). (2005). The nature of play: Great apes and humans. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Periss, V., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Trials and tribulations of childhood: An evolutionary perspective. In C. Salmon & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 149–168). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), (P. H. Mussen, Gen. Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Cognitive development (4th ed.) (pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierucci, J. M., O’Brien, C. T., McInnis, M. A., Gilpin, A. T., & Barber, A. B. (2014). Fantasy orientation constructs and related executive function development in preschool: Developmental benefits to executive functions by being a fantasy-oriented child. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 62–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., & Schwebel, D. C. (1997). Social and temperamental influences on children’s overestimation of their physical abilities: Links to accidental injuries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 317–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poirier, F. E., & Smith, E. O. (1974). Socializing functions of primate play. American Zoologist, 14, 275–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radesky, J. S., Silverstein, M., Zuckerman, B., & Christakis, D. A. (2014). Infant self-regulation and early childhood media exposure. Pediatrics, 133, e1172–e1178.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, H. H., Foley, M. A., & Gimpert, N. (2002). The role of collaborative planning in children’s source-monitoring errors and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 44–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2012). A tale of two schools: The promise of playful learning. In B. Falk (Ed.), Defending childhood: Keeping the promise of early education (pp. 24–47). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richert, R., Robb, M. B., Fender, J. G., & Wartella, E. (2010). Word learning from baby videos. Achieves of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 164, 432–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robb, M. B., Richert, R., & Wartella, E. (2009). Just a talking book? Word learning from watching baby videos. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 27–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. (2011). I remember me: Mnemonic self-reference effects in preschool children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(3), Serial No. 300

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, G., & Dicke, U. (2005). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 249–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimelia, P., Michele, E., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Infant and Child Development, 21, 411–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rovee-Collier, C., & Shyi, C.-W. G. (1992). A functional and cognitive analysis of infant long-term retention. In M. L. Howe, C. J. Brainerd, & V. F. Reyna (Eds.), Development of long-term retention. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2013). Development of attention networks. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 683–705). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1988). Education for young children living in poverty: Child-initiated learning or teacher-directed instruction? The Elementary School Journal, 89, 212–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your life. New York, NY: Pocket Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellers, P. D., II, & Bjorklund, D. F. (2014). The development of adaptive memory. In B. L. Schwartz, M. L. Howe, M. P. Toglia, & H. Otgaar (Eds.), What’s adaptive about adaptive memory? (pp. 286–307). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shin, H.-E., Bjorklund, D. F., & Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive nature of children’s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognitive Development, 22, 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, A., & Riggs, K. J. (2011). Three- and 4-year-olds encode modeled actions in two ways leading to immediate imitation and delayed emulation. Developmental Psychology, 47, 834–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spinath, B., & Spinath, F. M. (2005). Development of self-perceived ability in elementary school: The role of parents’ perceptions, teacher evaluations, and intelligence. Cognitive Development, 20, 190–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. (1984). Young children’s performance expectations: Logical analysis or wishful thinking? In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 3. The development of achievement motivation (pp. 33–56). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Daniels, D. (1988). Declining perceptions of competence: A consequence of changes in the child or the educational environment? Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 352–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Blyer, P., Ryan, R., Milburn, S., & Salmon, J. M. (1998). Good beginnings: What difference does the program make in preparing children for school. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19, 41–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Daniels, D., & Milburn, S. (1995). Effects of different instructional approaches on young children’s achievement and motivation. Child Development, 66, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 1–98. Serial No. 226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Walter, V., Gampe, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Limitations to the cultural ratchet effect in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 152–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 37–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. In Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 331–350). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Turkewitz, G., & Kenny, P. (1982). Limitations on input as a basis for neural organization and perceptual development: A preliminary theoretical statement. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 357–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, D. S., Kittredge, A. K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkof, R. M., & Klahr, D. (2015). Making play work for education. Phi Delta Kappan, 98, 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 364, 2417–2428.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers’ understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42, 723–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, R. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Own and others’ prior experiences influence children’s imitation of causal acts. Cognitive Development, 26, 260–268.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2013). Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning. Cognition, 127, 203–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch-screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 13–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 553–574). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151, 364–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David F. Bjorklund .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bjorklund, D.F., Beers, C. (2016). The Adaptive Value of Cognitive Immaturity: Applications of Evolutionary Developmental Psychology to Early Education. In: Geary, D., Berch, D. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics