Skip to main content

Empirical Study: Discrimination in Personnel Selection?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Corporate Social Responsibility and Discrimination

Part of the book series: CSR, Sustainability, Ethics & Governance ((CSEG))

  • 2106 Accesses

Abstract

It was suggested the following factors perpetuate discrimination of women in a male-dominated setting: Gender stereotypes have personnel decision-makers implicitly associate desired and required traits of a job with men to a greater degree than with women. This pattern will reflect on ascriptions of competence and suitability to the advantage of men over women. The desire to surround oneself with and to trust more readily individuals similar to oneself—especially with challenging assignments—is expected to reflect negatively on women’s assessment of suitability in a male-dominated and stereotypically masculine-connoted context. Reference to the law in an organizational context without enforcement threat will not counter biases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Castilla and Benard (2010), p 543ff.

  2. 2.

    Nebenzahl et al. (1993), p 101ff; Eagly et al. (1995), p 136ff.

  3. 3.

    Castilla and Benard (2010), p 543ff.

  4. 4.

    Wennerås and Wold (1997); Fernandez and Mors (2008); Shantz et al. (2011).

  5. 5.

    Strunk et al. (2005) in: Mayrhofer et al. (2005), p 211ff.

  6. 6.

    Charness (2010), p 21f.

  7. 7.

    Walker and Willer in: Webster and Sell (2007), p 25f.

  8. 8.

    Hegtvedt in: Webster and Sell (2007), p 142.

  9. 9.

    Polzer et al. (1998), p 42ff.

  10. 10.

    Webster and Sell in: Webster and Sell (2007), p 245f.

  11. 11.

    TU Career Center, http://www.tucareer.com/SiteContents/page/de/startseite

  12. 12.

    The timeframe studied was from January 2006 to February 2010.

  13. 13.

    Früh (2007), p 62ff.

  14. 14.

    Auer-Srnka and Koeszegi (2007), p 36.

  15. 15.

    Früh (2007), p 86f.

  16. 16.

    Früh (2007), p 128.

  17. 17.

    Cohen’s Kappa was chosen as the preferable measure, Auer-Srnka and Koeszegi (2007), p 44ff. Inter-coder reliability turned out to be highly satisfying with a Kappa value of 97 %.

  18. 18.

    Auer-Srnka and Koeszegi (2007), p 36ff.

  19. 19.

    It was agreed by the coders that a mere referral to expectations selected candidates be involved in “teaching and research” was not sufficient as these tasks go without saying for a job in academia. Only when this threshold was exceeded through a more substantiated description of future tasks and responsibilities, the unit was coded under this category of job or task description.

  20. 20.

    A tricky decision regarding the coding manual for the technical skill category was how to deal with IT skills. Undoubtedly, IT skills, for example knowledge or skills related to specialized software or methods involving information technology, are technical skills. Following the general coding rule, the more specific category was relevant. Since IT skills is more specific than technical skills, any requests for IT knowledge or skills were to be coded under IT. For the case of job advertisings related to the school of Information Technology, such a coding strategy would skew findings towards frequencies of IT skills and to the disadvantage of technical skills (of the specific discipline). IT graduates’ technical skills will most likely always be IT skills. For the sake of comparability across schools, technical skills from the school of Information Technology’s ads were defined as technical skills just like all other subject and discipline-related skills of other disciplines were coded as technical skills. This strategy, whilst keeping requests for technical skills comparable across schools, of course led to the odd phenomenon that Information Technology job ads were to have all frequencies of discipline-related skills under “technical”, and none under “IT” skills—a peculiarity deemed acceptable for the sake of consistency and comparability.

  21. 21.

    The remaining advertisings meant to employ under-graduate members for administrative tasks within the institution or for early research activity in the framework of research projects.

  22. 22.

    Compare Steffens and Mehl (2003) for details and references.

  23. 23.

    Heilman et al. (1989), Schein (2001).

  24. 24.

    Steffens and Mehl (2003), p 173ff; Webster and Sell (2007), p 229.

  25. 25.

    Landy and Farr (1980), p 72ff; Swim et al. (1989), p 423.

  26. 26.

    Eagly et al. (1995), p 125ff.

  27. 27.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 249.

  28. 28.

    Dubinsky and Rudelius (1980) in: Bagozzi et al.

  29. 29.

    Stedham et al. (2007), p 5.

  30. 30.

    Petersen and Krings (2009), p 506ff.

  31. 31.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 75.

  32. 32.

    Powell (1987), Huffcutt and Roth (1998), Rosenstein and Hitt (1986).

  33. 33.

    Swim et al. (1989), p 423.

  34. 34.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 6ff.

  35. 35.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 294.

  36. 36.

    Scholarios and Lockyer (1999), p 142.

  37. 37.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 294.

  38. 38.

    Larwood et al. (1988), p 14

  39. 39.

    Güth and Kliemt (2010), p 46.

  40. 40.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 493.

  41. 41.

    Dabic and Hatzinger in: Hatzinger et al. (2009), p 131.

  42. 42.

    Koordinationsstelle für Frauenförderung und Gender Studies, TU Wien, http://www.tuwien.ac.at/dle/koostelle/daten_und_fakten/hoererinnenzahlen/

  43. 43.

    To make the identical form of submissions believable, subjects were given information applications had to be submitted via the institute’s own online tool, which provided a framework for the inclusion of information, and which produced identical layout for all applications.

  44. 44.

    Steffens and Mehl (2003), p 173ff.

  45. 45.

    Webster and Sell (2007), p 75f.

  46. 46.

    Military service in Austria is mandatory for male citizens, and voluntary for female citizens.

  47. 47.

    Charness (2010), p 21f.

  48. 48.

    Webster and Sell in: Webster and Sell (2007), p 13.

  49. 49.

    Powell (1987); Fernandez and Mors (2008); Castilla and Benard (2010).

  50. 50.

    Larwood et al. (1988), p 14.

  51. 51.

    McCabe et al. (2006), p 105.

  52. 52.

    Schools differed in the proportion of female scientists and students: Mechanical Engineering participated with 18.18 % of female senior scientists (at a very low response rate of senior scientists, though), and with 16.47 % of female students; Civil Engineering had 100 % male senior scientists responding (again, at a very low senior scientist response rate) and 32.65 % participation of female students; 27.59 % of senior scientists in Technical Chemistry were women as well as 32.26 % of students from this school.

  53. 53.

    Weber and Weiler in: Hatzinger et al. (2009), p 49ff; Dabic and Hatzinger in: Hatzinger et al. (2009), p 119ff.

  54. 54.

    An advantage of ranked orders over directly paired comparisons is subjects may inflate scores by according equally or similarly high ratings to sets of pairs, thereby avoiding clear choices beyond the immediately contrasted pair (Krosnicka and Alwin 1988, p 534).

  55. 55.

    Krosnick and Alwin (1988), p 534.

  56. 56.

    Heilman (1997), p 882.

  57. 57.

    Swim et al. (1989), p 410.

  58. 58.

    Basow (1986), p 60ff.

  59. 59.

    Porter and Roberts (1976), p 1559ff.

  60. 60.

    Szwajkowsi and Larwood (1991), p 512.

  61. 61.

    Metz (2003), p 240f.

  62. 62.

    Petersen and Krings (2009), p 508f.

  63. 63.

    Powell (1987), Huffcutt and Roth (1998).

  64. 64.

    Früh (2007), p 27f.

  65. 65.

    Früh (2007), p 63.

  66. 66.

    Hellerstein et al. (2002), p 714.

  67. 67.

    The example statement “candidate X does not excel in languages like candidate Y” would be unitized into two different sense units. Unit 1 would read “candidate X does not excel in languages (like candidate Y)”. A corresponding (split) unit 2 from this originally integrated statement would read “(candidate X does not) excel in languages like candidate Y (does)”. In contrast to this, a statement like “candidate X excels in languages” would be one thought unit. For the permissibility of this technique of adding or reducing semantic fillers without altering the content, see also: Früh (2007), p 92f.

  68. 68.

    Against this methodological choice for their concrete example, but in principle recognizing the permissibility of this choice for designs that intend to measure increased intensity of an argument: Früh (2007), p 92f.

  69. 69.

    The preliminary category system was developed with about 10 % of the unitized. Compare: Früh (2007), p 156f.

  70. 70.

    Main categories unrelated to immediate arguments on applicant suitability were (7) need for clarification of doubts regarding candidates in a job interview, (8) critique of the study or its design, and (9) other.

  71. 71.

    Früh (2007), p 207, p 242f.

  72. 72.

    Früh (2007), p 73.

  73. 73.

    Heilman et al. (1995), Schein (2001), Eagly et al. (1995).

  74. 74.

    A manual ensuring discriminatory power and invariability of the coding rules was prepared in a way that reliability would not be increased at the cost of validity, meaning explicit examples were not used at this stage in order not to render coding a rather mechanical task that would ensure incomplete capture “with great precision”, but defeat interpretative accomplishment by coders. Compare Früh (2007), p 87ff.

  75. 75.

    Früh (2007), p 129.

  76. 76.

    Früh (2007), p 188ff, 263.

  77. 77.

    In absolute frequencies, 145 of 204 and 194 of 253 arguments from the six applicant characteristics categories are quality/“plus” mentions for the gender-blind and gender-visible responses, respectively.

  78. 78.

    Note as mentioned in Appendix C, there is reason to depart from a strict 0.05 threshold for significance considerations in the case of (conservative) Fisher test.

  79. 79.

    Nieva and Gutek (1980), p 273f.

References

  • Basow SA (1986) Gender stereotypes: traditions and alternatives. Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Früh W (2007) Inhaltsanalyse: Theorie und Praxis6. Utb GmbH, Konstanz

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayrhofer W, Meyer M, Steyrer J (eds) (2005) Macht? Erfolg? Reich? Glücklich? Einflussfaktoren auf Karrieren. Linde, Wien

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster M Jr, Sell J (eds) (2007) Laboratory experiments in the social sciences. Academic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Castilla EJ, Benard S (2010) The paradox of meritocracy in organizations. Adm Sci Q 55:543–576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness G (2010) Laboratory experiments: challenges and promise a review of “theory and experiment: what are the questions?” by Vernon Smith. J Econ Behav Organ 73:21–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubinsky AJ, Rudelius W (1980) Ethical beliefs: how students compare with industrial salespeople. In: Bagozzi RP (ed) Marketing in the 80’s: changes and challenges. American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, pp 73–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly AH, Karau SJ, Makhijani MG (1995) Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 117:125–145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez RM, Mors ML (2008) Competing for jobs: labor queues and gender sorting in the hiring process. Soc Sci Res 37:1061–1080

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Güth W, Kliemt H (2010) Comments on Vernon Smith’s-theory and experiment: what are the questions. J Econ Behav Organ 73(1):44–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatzinger R, Dittrich R, Salzberger T (2009) Präferenzanalyse mit R—Anwendungen aus marketing, Behavioural finance und human resource management. Facultas, Wien

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman ME, Block CJ, Martell RF, Simon MC (1989) Has anything changed? current characterizations of men, women, and managers. J Appl Psychol 74(6):935–942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman ME, Block CJ, Martell RF, Simon MC (1995) Sex stereotypes: do the influence perceptions of managers? J Soc Behav Pers 10(6):237–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Hellerstein JK, Neumark D, Troske KR (2002) Market forces and sex discrimination. J Hum Resour 37(2):353–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huffcutt AI, Roth PL (1998) Racial group differences in employment interview evaluations. J Appl Psychol 83(2):179–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick JA, Alwin DF (1988) A test of the form-resistant correlation hypothesis: ratings, rankings, and the measurement of values. Public Opin Q 52(4):526–538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landy FJ, Farr JL (1980) Performance rating. Psychol Bull 87(1):72–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larwood L, Szwajkowski E, Rose S (1988) Sex and race discrimination resulting from manager-client relationships: applying the rational bias theory of managerial discrimination. Sex Roles 18(1–2):9–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe C, Ingram R, Conway DM (2006) The business of ethics and gender. J Bus Ethics 64(2):101–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metz I (2003) Individual, interpersonal, and organisational links to women’s advancement in management in banks. Women Manag Rev 18(5):236–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nebenzahl ID, Jaffe ED, Gotesdyner H (1993) Perceptions of Israeli male and female managerial behavior in small group. Int Stud Manag Organ 23(3):97–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieva VF, Gutek BA (1980) Sex effects on evaluation. Acad Manag Rev 5(2):267–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen LE, Krings F (2009) Are ethical codes of conduct toothless tigers for dealing with employment discrimination? J Bus Ethics 85(4):501–514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polzer JT, Mannix EA, Neale MA (1998) Interest alignment in coalitions in multiparty negotiations. Acad Manag J 41(1):42–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter LW, Roberts KH (1976) Communication in organizations. In: Dunnette MD (ed) Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. Rand McNally, Chicago, IL, pp 1553–1589

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell GN (1987) The effects of sex and gender on recruitment. Acad Manag J 12(4):731–743

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenstein J, Hitt MA (1986) Experimental research on race and sex discrimination: the record and the prospects. J Occup Behav 7:215–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schein VE (2001) A global look at psychological barriers to women’s progress in management. J Soc Issues 57:675–688

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholarios D, Lockyer C (1999) Recruiting and selecting professionals: context, qualities and methods. Int J Sel Assess 7:142–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shantz A, Wright K, Latham G (2011) Networking with boundary spanners: a quasi-case study on why women are less likely to be offered an engineering role. Equality Divers Inclusion Int J 30(3):217–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stedham Y, Yamamura JH, Beekun RI (2007) Gender differences in business ethics: justice and relativist perspectives. Bus Ethics Eur Rev 16(2):163–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens MC, Mehl B (2003) “Erscheinen Karrierefrauen” weniger kompetent als “Karrieremänner”? Geschlechterstereotype und Kompetenzuschreibung. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie 34(3):173–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swim J, Borgida E, Maruyama G, Myers DG (1989) Joan McKay versus John McKay: do gender stereotypes bias evaluations? Psychol Bull 105(3):409–429

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szwajkowski E, Larwood L (1991) Rational decision processes and sex discrimination: testing “rational” bias theory. J Organ Behav 12(6):507–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wennerås C, Wold A (1997) Nepotism and sexism in peer-review. Nature 387(22):341–343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Auer-Srnka KJ, Koeszegi S (2007) From words to numbers: how to transform qualitative data into meaningful quantitative results. Schmalenbach Bus Rev 59:29–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman ME (1997) Sex discrimination and the affirmative action remedy: the role of sex stereotypes. J Bus Ethics 16:877–889

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Keinert-Kisin, C. (2016). Empirical Study: Discrimination in Personnel Selection?. In: Corporate Social Responsibility and Discrimination. CSR, Sustainability, Ethics & Governance. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29158-1_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics