Keywords

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Introduction

With a half a second’s planning, we rapidly carry out complex sequences of movements. Motor planning refers to our ability to conceive, plan, and initiate a skilled motor behavior. During motor planning, sensory information must be integrated to inform the appropriate motor responses. Behaviorally relevant information must be kept in short-term memory. Thus the process of motor planning taps into multiple aspects of flexible behavior and offers a way of studying the mental processes that ultimately culminate in a movement, hence a window into cognition.

A neural correlate of motor planning was first reported in humans as a deflection in EEG recordings, over motor and parietal cortex, which anticipates voluntary movements (a.k.a. Bereitschaftspotential; Deecke et al. 1976). The EEG signal appears long before the onset of the movement and hundreds of milliseconds before the subjects are aware of their desire to move (Libet 1985). The neural correlates of motor planning were discovered in the primate motor cortex by Tanji and Evarts (1976), who described neurons that discharged persistently before an instructed movement. This persistent activity ramped up shortly after the instruction, long before the movement onset, and predicted specific types of future movements. These findings opened the possibility of studying the mechanisms of motor planning at the level of neural circuits (Riehle and Requin 1989; Crutcher and Alexander 1990; Turner and DeLong 2000; Shenoy et al. 2013).

Behavioral paradigms in rodents are rapidly developing, and it is possible to train mice in behavioral tasks that dissociate planning and movement in time, analogous to the tasks used in primates (Guo et al. 2014a, b). The mouse is a genetically tractable organism, providing access to defined cell types for recordings and perturbations (Luo et al. 2008; O’Connor et al. 2009). In addition, the lissencephalic macrostructure of the mouse brain allows unobstructed access to a large fraction of the brain for functional analysis. We study motor planning in the context of a tactile decision behavior (Guo et al. 2014a; Li et al. 2015). Mice measure the location of an object using their whiskers and report their judgment by directional licking. We delineate when and how activity in specific cortical regions areas drives the tactile decision behavior in mice. New recording and perturbation methods are beginning to reveal the circuit mechanisms underlying motor planning that, in turn, will shed light on the biophysics of flexible behavior.

Head-Fixed Tactile Decision Behavior and Involved Cortical Regions

We developed a tactile decision task to track the flow of information in cortex during motor planning (Guo et al. 2014a; Li et al. 2015). Head-fixed mice measured the location of a pole using their whiskers and reported their decisions about object location with directional licking (Fig. 1a). In each trial, the pole was presented in one of two positions (anterior or posterior) during a sample epoch (1.3 s; Fig. 1b). During a subsequent delay epoch (1.3 s), the mice planned the upcoming response. An auditory go cue (0.1 s) signaled the beginning of the response epoch, when the mice reported the perceived pole position by licking one of two lickports (posterior → “lick right”, anterior → “lick left”). The mice achieved high levels of performance (~80 % correct). Responses before the go cue were rare (~13 %). They performed the tactile decision behavior across many sessions (up to 85 sessions, >400 trials per session), yielding tens of thousands of trials per mouse. The large number of trials allowed us to use optogenetic silencing to identify the cortical regions involved in an unbiased manner on a cortex-wide scale.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mapping the cortical regions underlying tactile decision behavior. (a) Head-fixed mouse responding “lick right” or “lick left” based on pole location. (b) The pole was within reach during the sample epoch. Mice responded with licking after a delay and an auditory go cue. (c) Fifty-five cortical locations were tested in loss-of-function experiments during different behavioral epochs. Top, photoinhibition during sample (left) and delay (right) epochs. Bottom, cortical regions involved in the tactile decision behavior during sample (left) and delay (right) epochs in “lick right” trials. Color codes for the change in performance (%) under photoinhibition relative to control performance. Circle size codes for significance (p values, from small to large; >0.025, <0.025, <0.01, <0.001). Figure adapted from Guo et al. (2014a)

To achieve powerful cortical inactivation, we used transgenic mice expressing Channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in cortical GABAergic interneurons. This photoinhibition silenced millimeter-size tissue volumes by photostimulating through the intact skull (85 % activity reduction) with time resolutions on the order of 100 ms. We developed a scanning laser system to survey the neocortex for regions driving behavior during specific behavioral epochs. First, we outfitted the mice with a clear-skull cap preparation that provided optical access to half of the neocortex. A scanning system targeted photostimuli in a random access manner. Head-fixation and precise control of the laser position allowed each mouse to be tested repeatedly across multiple behavioral sessions. We tested 55 evenly spaced cortical volumes in sensory, motor, and parietal cortex for their involvement in the behavior by applying photoinhibition during specific behavioral epochs (Fig. 1c).

Inactivation of most cortical volumes did not cause any behavioral change. Inactivating vibrissal primary somatosensory cortex (vS1, “barrel cortex”) caused deficits in object location discrimination. The effect was temporally specific: inactivation during the delay epoch produced a much smaller deficit, suggesting that tactile information was transferred out of the vS1 during the sample epoch (Fig. 1c). During the delay epoch, preceding the motor response, inactivation of an anterior lateral region of the motor cortex (ALM) biased the upcoming movement (Fig. 1c). We used silicon probes to record single units from vS1 and ALM in mice performing the tactile decision behavior. Single unit recordings supported the photoinhibition experiments: a large fraction of neurons in vS1 showed object location-dependent activity during the sample epoch, whereas the majority of neurons in ALM showed movement-specific preparatory activity and peri-movement during the delay and response epochs. These results begin to outline the information flow in mouse cortex involved in the tactile decision behavior. The information flow is largely consistent with a serial scheme, where information is passed from sensory areas to motor areas during motor planning (Guo et al. 2014a).

Premotor Dynamics Underlying Motor Planning

A large fraction (>70 %) of ALM neurons significantly distinguished upcoming movements during at least one of the trial epochs (p < 0.05, two-tailed t-test; Fig. 2a). On error trials, when mice licked in the opposite direction to the instruction provided by object location (Fig. 1a), most ALM neuron activities predicted the licking direction (Guo et al. 2014a; Li et al. 2015). Such movement-specific activity is consistent with a role in planning and driving movements (Fig. 1c). Interestingly, the preparatory activity in ALM was not static but evolved with complex dynamics: subsets of neurons showed selective activity during the sample epoch whereas other neurons showed “bumps” of activity during different times of the delay epoch (Fig. 2a). Despite these fluctuating responses at the level of the single neurons, selectivity for the upcoming movement remained stable at the level of the population (Fig. 2b). On average, selectivity emerged in the sample epoch and ramped up throughout the delay epoch, reaching a maximum at the beginning of the response epoch (Fig. 2b). This ramping activity in ALM during the delay epoch is similar to the ramping activity reported in frontal (Hanes and Schall 1996; Murakami et al. 2014), parietal (Roitman and Shadlen 2002; Maimon and Assad 2006; Harvey et al. 2012), and motor cortex (Erlich et al. 2011; Thura and Cisek 2014), anticipating voluntary movements in primates and rodents. The ALM preparatory activity could provide a substrate for the maintenance of the motor plan during the delay epoch. Information about the upcoming movement is likely coded at the level of the population (Laurent 2002; Harvey et al. 2012; Shenoy et al. 2013; Murakami and Mainen 2015). Consistent with a distributed code, we found that spatially intermingled ALM neurons in each hemisphere have a preference for either contra- or ipsi-lateral movements in roughly equal proportions (Li et al. 2015).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Head-fixed tactile decision behavior and involved cortical regions. (a) Nine examples of ALM neurons. Top, spike raster. Bottom, PSTH. Correct “lick right” (blue) and “lick left” (red) trials only. Dashed lines, behavioral epochs. (b) Average ALM population selectivity in spike rate (± s.e.m. across neurons, bootstrap). Selectivity is the difference in spike rate between the preferred and non-preferred movements. Time zero is the onset of the go cue. Averaging window, 200 ms

The preparatory activity encodes upcoming movement, yet it does not cause movement during planning. How does preparatory activity evolve into commands that descend to motor centers to trigger movement? Paradoxically, ALM neurons in each hemisphere have a preference for contra- or ipsi-lateral movements in roughly equal proportions, yet unilateral inactivation of ALM biased the upcoming movement to the ipsi-lateral direction. How does silencing a brain area with non-lateralized selectivity cause a directional movement bias? We measured neuronal activity within hierarchically organized ALM circuits. ALM projection neurons include two major classes: intratelencephalic (IT) neurons that project to other cortical areas and pyramidal tract (PT) neurons that project out of the cortex, including to motor-related areas in the brainstem (Komiyama et al. 2010; Shepherd 2013). IT neurons connect to other IT neurons and excite PT neurons, but not vice versa. PT neurons are thus at the output end of the local ALM circuit (Morishima and Kawaguchi 2006; Brown and Hestrin 2009; Kiritani et al. 2012; Shepherd 2013). We recorded activity from identified IT and PT neurons using cell-type specific electrophysiology and two photon imaging. We found that ALM IT neurons have mixed preparatory activity for both ipsi- and contra-lateral movements. Contra-lateral population activity in PT neurons arose late during the delay epoch to drive directional licking. To test the causal role of the PT neuron population activity in driving movements, we manipulated PT neurons by expressing ChR2 in mouse lines selectively expressing cre in these neurons. Weak activation of the PT neurons during movement planning could “write-in” specific motor plans that resulted in contra-lateral licking movements. These results suggest that, during movement planning, distributed preparatory activity in IT neuron networks is converted into a movement command in PT neurons (‘output-potent’ activity; Kaufman et al. 2014), which ultimately triggers directional movements (Li et al. 2015).

Open Questions

Several key questions remain unsolved. What circuit mechanisms are responsible for the maintenance of motor plan during short-term memory? How is sensory information integrated into the motor plan? How do the basal ganglia and motor thalamus interact with cortical regions during motor planning? Answering these questions will require recordings and manipulation of specific cell types. Importantly, architectural and cell type information must be incorporated into models of cortical dynamics. Tools to manipulate projections between brain regions are needed to study the interactions between brain regions. Finally, there is still a long way to go in developing richer behavioral paradigms that tap into the capabilities of the mammalian brain.