Skip to main content

Licensing Internal Arguments

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Building Predicates

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT,volume 92))

  • 232 Accesses

Abstract

Continuing the investigation of grammatical relations in Palauan, this chapter focuses on direct objects, which exhibit an unusual pattern of accusative case morphology. I begin by demonstrating that the aspectual properties of transitive verbs correlate with differences in how their direct object DPs are marked, one of which is a dependent-marking differential object marking pattern, and the other is a head-marking object agreement pattern. I present a wealth of evidence from raising-to-object constructions, morphological causatives, transitive stative predicates, and passives that—despite the aspectual split and difference between the case-marking patterns—transitive predicates are structurally identical. I develop an analysis of direct object DP licensing and the syntactic composition of aspect in the verbal complex, concluding that there are different “flavors” of transitive v that are specified for different aspect features. I propose that direct objects are uniformly licensed via an Agree relation instantiated by transitive v and present evidence for this view with data involving left-conjunct agreement. The conclusions drawn in this chapter serve to augment our knowledge of the features of argument structure, Case licensing, aspect, and object agreement in Palauan and cross-linguistically.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I show in Sect. 3.3 that when er appears overtly to mark direct objects of imperfective verbs, it is not a preposition, but rather a simple case-marker that is inserted post-Spell Out, on the PF branch.

  2. 2.

    The verbs in (113) and (114) are prime examples of the complex verbal morphophonology of Palauan. To understand how these verbs are formed, it might help to think of perfective verbalization as the infixation of -m- into the roots. In (113), we would have \(\sqrt{\textsc {kal}}\) \(+\) -m- which results in the intermediate representation /kmal/. In (113a), the suffixation of -ii results in a shift in stress to the final syllable, which triggers coalescence of /ma/ into /o/, resulting in the surface form kolii. In (113b) on the other hand, no suffixation occurs, so the intermediate form /kmal/ is a monosyllable with main stress, allowing the word-final /l/ to drop, resulting in /kma/. Finally, the velar nasal—which optionally suffixes to words ending in /a/, /o/, or /u/—is inserted, resulting in the surface form kmang. In (114), the verb stems both host suffixes that attract stress, so when -m- is infixed into the root \(\sqrt{\textsc {ues}}\) to yield the intermediate forms /umes-a/ and /umes-te-rir/, coalescence of /um/ before /e/ in an unstressed syllable results in /m/, yielding the surface forms mesterir and mesang, which again shows suffixation of the velar nasal. Thanks to the careful work of Wilson (1972a, b ) and Flora (1974 ), such intricacies of Palauan morphophonology are now well-understood and derivable, despite their complexity.

  3. 3.

    While this set of object agreement suffixes is compatible with the vast majority of Palauan perfective verbs, there is nevertheless a relatively large subclass of irregular verbs which show some variability in the form of their object agreement suffixes, typically in the 3rd person. This is one of the reasons that Georgopoulos (1991 ) analyzes them as true affixes rather than clitics. An example of an irregular form is the [3sg] suffix -ang in mesang “see” in (114a).

  4. 4.

    See for example Tenny (1987, 1994 ) , Krifka (1992 ) , Travis (1992, 2005, 2010 ) , Ramchand (1997 ) , Arad (1998a, b ) , Ritter and Rosen (2000 ) , Kratzer (2004 ) and Coon (2013 ) .

  5. 5.

    Note the homophony between Palauan’s only preposition, er, and the accusative case marker er. It is not uncommon crosslinguistically for languages to utilize/reanalyze prepositional, locative, or dative morphemes as accusative case markers in differential object marking systems—a fact that presumably calls for some explanation. In addition to Spanish personal a, which marks human direct objects and is homophonous with the preposition a (see (169) through (171) later in this chapter), an anonymous reviewer provides some other examples. In Hindi, the postposition -ko marks both recipients in ditransitive clauses as well as highly animate/specific themes in monotransitives. In Malagasy, pronouns, proper names of humans, and (optionally) DPs introduced by a demonstrative take the proclitic an- when they occur in direct object position; an- also functions as a locative marker. These patterns of homophony might have some structural basis, or they might be the by-product of a common grammaticalization path for the creation of differential object marking. Unfortunately, I cannot say anything interesting or intelligent about the rationale behind the homophony between the differential object marker er in Palauan and the preposition er. It is my hope that future research can uncover the reason for this homophony in language after language.

  6. 6.

    Fortin (2006 ) also argues that Indonesian meng- is a 3rd person indefinite clitic object pronoun that acts as an antipassive marker. Her analysis accounts for some key phenomena in Indonesian, but an antipassive analysis is unlikely to be tenable for Palauan meN-, as meN- appears even on verbs with derived objects like causatives and raising-to-object verbs, as described below.

  7. 7.

    Raising-to-object constructions are also known as exceptional case marking (ECM) constructions, depending on the theoretical assumptions one makes. See Rosenbaum (1967 ) , Kiparsky and Kiparsky (1970 ) , Chomsky (1973, 1981 ) , Postal (1974 ) , Lasnik and Saito (1991 ) and Runner (1995, 1998, 2006 ) .

  8. 8.

    Note the word order in (147) and (148), in which the (bracketed) embedded subject appears directly to the right of the verb. The word order appears to be the result of clause extraposition, like that seen in (90) and (91) in Chap. 2, but unfortunately I do not have clean evidence to support that analysis. What is important, however, is that the embedded subjects receive \(\theta \)-roles from the embedded predicates, but trigger object agreement on the matrix verb.

  9. 9.

    I remain agnostic as to whether a raising-to-object analysis or an ECM analysis is superior. Although it does seem clear from the position of the case-marked DP that movement has occurred, there is not yet sufficient empirical evidence in Palauan that the moved DP is in the matrix clause for me to advocate one analysis over the other.

  10. 10.

    The name of such a Case is purely theoretical, as the morphology indicating it is null and it is not clear what semantic contribution it would add. Calling it Partitive is arbitrary; it could also be called Dative, or simply Inherent.

  11. 11.

    Note that Josephs (1990: 364) lists meiko and muiko for “blind,” while Ramarui and Temael (1999: 221) list only meiko. As the correct spelling of the Palauan word for “blind” in (152a) is controversial, I have (without consequence) chosen to leave my consultant’s spelling of mikeiu intact, should this somehow be of use to future researchers. The important point to take away from this sentence is not the spelling of any particular word but rather that the derived object a mikeiu el chad “the blind person” is [\(+\) hum, sg, \(+\) spec] and is marked with er.

  12. 12.

    Recall the Persian examples in (126) and (127), the Finnish examples in (128), the Turkish examples in (129), and the Amharic examples in (130). The alternating structural/inherent Case analysis might be a good candidate to explain the facts in these languages.

  13. 13.

    The meN-/oN- prefixes are Palauan’s reflexes of pan-Austronesian (or at least pan-Malayo-Polynesian) maN-, with a change of Proto *n > l, which explains the appearance of [l] instead of [n] in applications of nasal substitution with alveolar inputs. The two forms meN- and oN- are phonologically conditioned allomorphs, alternating predictably according to the shapes of the stems to which they attach. The set of infix allomorphs represents Palauan’s instantiation of pan-Austronesian/ Malayo-Polynesian -um-.

  14. 14.

    Palauan uek- is presumably cognate with Tagalog pag-.

  15. 15.

    The details of the morphophonology are tangential to the current discussion. But to be precise, I analyze the imperfective verb omekdakt in (154a) as oN- \(+\) uek- \(+\) \(\sqrt{\textsc {dakt}}\). The nasal substitution induced by oN- results in /u/ \(\rightarrow \) /m/. I analyze the perfective verb mekdektii in (154b) as -m- \(+\) uek- \(+\) \(\sqrt{\textsc {dakt}}\) \(+\) -ii. The infixation of -m- into uek- results in the sequence /umek/, in which /um/ undergoes coalescence and becomes simply /m/. Even if my morphological analysis turns out to be flawed, what is crucial is that the causative prefix does not appear outside of the aspect morphology, yielding (unattested) forms like *uek-melakt (imperfective) or *uek-dmakt (perfective).

  16. 16.

    Travis (2010 : 268) uses slightly different terminology. She says that imparfait selects a homogeneous expression, whereas passé simple selects a quantized expression. What is important for the present discussion is simply that there is a selectional relation that targets a particular aspect.

  17. 17.

    In Sect. 3.4.2 I called the features that appear on Asp\({}_{\mathrm {s}}\) [ ±definite/telic]; I am not committed to a particular label for the features that occupy the Asp\({}_{\mathrm {s}}\) position, and as their associated morphology is null, any label would be purely theoretical.

  18. 18.

    Travis’s (2010 : 53–62) discussion of Tagalog reduplication is what reminded me of the Palauan reduplication data in Josephs (1997 : 375–380.)

  19. 19.

    See Wagner (2005, 2010 ) for additional prosodic evidence that&P might be asymmetrical, at least in some languages.

  20. 20.

    See also Corbett (1979, 1983, 1988 ) for extensive work on resolution rules for coordinate structures in Slavic.

  21. 21.

    V could also easily be called \(\sqrt{\textsc {root}}\) if one adopts the category-neutral root theory of Marantz (1997 ) et seq.

References

  • Ahland, Michael. 2006. Complementation in Amharic: Degrees of finiteness and integration. Paper presented at the 34th North American Conference on Afroasiatic Languages, Seattle, 17 March.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity versus economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21: 435–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arad, Maya. 1998a. VP-structure and the syntax-lexicon interface. Doctoral Dissertation, University College London. Published as Arad 1998b.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arad, Maya. 1998b. VP-structure and the syntax-lexicon interface. Cambridge: MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark. 1985. The mirror principle and morphosyntactic explanation. Linguistic Inquiry 16: 373–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bittner, Maria, and Ken Hale. 1996a. Ergativity: Toward a theory of heterogeneous class. Linguistic Inquiry 27: 531–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bittner, Maria, and Ken Hale. 1996b. The structural determination of case and agreement. Linguistic Inquiry 27: 1–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bossong, Georg. 1985. Empirische Universalienforschung: Differentielle Objektmarkierung in den neuiranischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, Joan. 1972. Theory of complementation in English syntax. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, Luigi. 1986. Italian syntax: A Government-Binding approach. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capell, Arthur. 1949. A grammar of the language of Palau. Coordinated Investigation of Micronesian Anthropology (CIMA) 1947–1949. Part 6b of the final report to the Pacific Science Board, National Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1973. Conditions on transformations. In A Festschrift for Morris Halle, eds. Stephen Anderson and Paul Kiparsky, 232–286. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by step: Essays on Minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka, 89–155. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 1–52. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra. 1998. The design of agreement: Evidence from Chamorro. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coon, Jessica. 2013. Aspects of split ergativity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cooreman, Ann. 1983. Chamorro texts. Ms., Saipan, Commonwealth of the Northern Mariana Islands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, Greville. 1979. Predicate agreement in Russian. Birmingham: University of Birmingham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, Greville. 1983. Hierarchies, targets, and controllers. London: Croon Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, Greville. 1988. Agreement: A partial specification based on Slavonic data. In Agreement in natural language: Approaches, theories, descriptions, eds. Michael Barlow and Charles Ferguson, 23–55. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demirdache, Hamida, and Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria. 2000. The primitives of temporal relations. In Step by step: Essays on Minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka, 157–186. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demirdache, Hamida, and Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria. 2004. The syntax of time adverbs. In The syntax of time, eds. Jacqueline Guéron and Jacqueline Lecarme, 143–179. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demirdache, Hamida, and Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria. 2005. Aspect and temporal modification. In Aspectual inquiries, eds. Paula Kempchinsky and Roumyana Slabakova, 191–221. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demonte, Violeta. 1987. C-command, prepositions and predication. Linguistic Inquiry 18: 147–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeWolf, Charles. 1988. Voice in Austronesian lanugages of Philippine type: Passive, ergative, or neither? In Passive and voice, ed. Masayoshi Shibatani, 143–193. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Swart, Henriëtte. 1998. Aspect shift and coercion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16: 347–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Swart, Peter. 2007. Cross-linguistic variation in object marking. Doctoral Dissertation, Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David. 1997. Voice and the interfaces of syntax. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David, and Rolf Noyer. 2001. Movement operations after syntax. Linguistic Inquiry 32: 555–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enç, Mürvet. 1991. The semantics of specificity. Linguistic Inquiry 22: 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flora, Jo-Ann. 1974. Palauan phonology and morphology. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortin, Catherine. 2006. Reconciling meng- and NP movement in Indonesian. Paper presented at the 32nd annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, February 11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, Carol. 1985. Variables in Palauan syntax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 59–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, Carol. 1991. Syntactic variables: Resumptive pronouns and A \(^{\prime }\) binding in Palauan. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2011. Positive polarity items and negative polarity items: Variation, licensing, and compositionality. In Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning, eds. Claudia Maienborn, Klaus von Heusinger, and Paul Portner, 1660–1712. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, Jane. 2005. Words and structure. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinämäki, Orvokki. 1984. Aspect in finnish. In Aspect bound: A voyage into the realm of Germanic, Slavonic, and Finno-Ugrian aspectology, eds. Casper de Groot and Hannu Tommola, 153–178. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Kyle. 1991. Object positions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 9: 577–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, Lewis. 1975. Palauan reference grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, Lewis. 1990. New Palauan-English dictionary. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, Lewis. 1997. Handbook of Palauan grammar, Vol. 1. Koror: Ministry of Education, Republic of Palau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, Hans, and Christian Rohrer. 1983. Tense in texts. In Meaning, use, and interpretation of language, eds. Rainer Bäuerle, Christoph Schwarze, and Arnim von Stechow, 250–269. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul, and Carol Kiparsky. 1970. Fact. In Progress in linguistics, eds. Manfred Bierwisch and Karl Erich Heidolph, 143–173. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the external argument from its verb. In Phrase structure and the lexicon, eds. Johan Rooryck and Laurie Zaring, 109–137. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 2004. Telicity and the meaning of objective case. In The syntax of time, eds. Jacqueline Guéron and Jacqueline Lecarme, 389–423. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krifka, Manfred. 1992. Thematic relations as links between nominal reference and temporal constitution. In Lexical matters, eds. Ivan Sag and Anna Szabolcsi, 29–53. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, William. 1979. Polarity sensitivity as inherent scope relations. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Texas, Austin. Published as Ladusaw 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, William. 1980. Polarity sensitivity as inherent scope relations. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard, and Mamoru Saito. 1991. On the subject of infinitives. In Papers from the 27th regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, eds. Lise Dobrin, Lynn Nichols, and Rosa M. Rodriguez, 324–343. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazard, Gilbert. 1982. Le morphème en Persan et les relations actancielles. Bulletin de la société de linguistique de Paris 73: 177–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legate, Julie Anne. 2008. Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry 39: 55–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 1997. No escape from syntax: Don’t try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon. In Proceedings of PLC 21, eds. Alexis Dimitriadis, Laura Siegel, Clarissa Surek-Clark, and Alexander Williams, 201–225. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Thomas. 2004. The position of morphological case in the derivation: A study on the syntax-morphology interface. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moens, Marc. 1993. Tense, aspect and temporal reference. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munn, Alan. 1993. Topics in the syntax and semantics of coordinate structures. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, Johanna. 1986. Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar. Language 62: 56–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, Matthew. 2001. The clause structure of Malagasy: A Minimalist approach. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, Matthew. 2012. Aspect and voice selection in Malagasy: Initial observations. In Theories of everything: In honor of Ed Keenan, eds. Thomas Graf, Denis Paperno, Anna Szabolcsi, and Jos Tellings, 337–347. Los Angeles: UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, David. 1978. Impersonal passives and the Unaccusative Hypothesis. In Proceedings of BLS 4, eds. Jeri Jaeger, Anthony Woodbury, Farrell Ackerman, Christine Chiarello, Orin Gensler, John Kingston, Eve Sweetser, Henry Thompson, and Kenneth Whitler, 157–189. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul. 1974. On raising. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramarui, Augusta, and Melii Temael. 1999. Kerresel a klechibelau: Tekoi er a Belau me a omesudel. Koror: Belau National Museum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, Gillian. 1997. Aspect and predication: The semantics of argument structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, Elizabeth, and Sara Rosen. 2000. Event structure and ergativity: The converging perspectives of lexical semantics and syntax. In Events as grammatical objects, eds. Carol Tenny and James Pustejovsky, 187–238. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi Luigi. 1990. Relativized minimality. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2001. Relativized minimality effects. In The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory, eds. Mark Baltin and Chris Collins, 89–110. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, Miguel. 2007. The syntax of objects: Agree and differential object marking. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Connecticut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, Peter. 1967. The grammar of English predicated complement constructions. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runner, Jeffrey T. 1995. Noun phrase licensing and interpretation. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Published as Runner 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runner, Jeffrey T. 1998. Noun phrase licensing. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runner, Jeffrey T. 2006. Lingering challenges to the raising-to-object and object-control constructions. Syntax 9: 193–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, Paul, and Fe Otanes. 1972. Tagalog reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schütze, Carson. 1997. INFL in child and adult language: Agreement, case and licensing. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, Carlota. 1991. The parameter of aspect. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Reprinted as Smith 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, Carlota. 1997. The parameter of aspect (2nd edition). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sneddon, James Neil. 1996. Indonesian: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stowell, Tim. 1995. The phrase structure of tense. In Phrase structure and the lexicon, eds. Johan Rooryck and Laurie Zaring, 277–292. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenny, Carol. 1987. Grammaticalizing aspect and affectedness. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenny, Carol. 1994. Aspectual roles and the syntax-semantics interface. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Topping, Donald. 1973. Chamorro reference grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, Lisa. 1992. Inner aspect and the structure of VP. Cahiers de Linguistique de l’UQAM 1: 130–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, Lisa. 2005. Articulated vPs and the composition of aspectual classes. In Aspectual inquiries, eds. Paula Kempchinsky and Roumyana Slabakova, 69–94. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, Lisa. 2010. Inner aspect: The articulation of VP. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vendler, Zeno. 1967. Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, Michael. 2005. Prosody and recursion. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, Michael. 2010. Prosody and recursion in coordinate structures and beyond. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28: 183–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Helen. 1972a. The phonology and syntax of Palauan verb affixes. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Hawaii. Published as Wilson 1972b.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Helen. 1972b. The phonology and syntax of Palauan verb affixes. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Working Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zagona, Karen. 1994. Perfectivity and temporal arguments. In Issues and theory in Romance Linguistics: Selected papers from the Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages XXIII, ed. Michael L. Mazzola, 523–546. Washington: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zagona, Karen. 2002. The syntax of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zoerner, Cyril Edward. 1995. Coordination: The syntax of&P. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Irvine.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Justin Nuger .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nuger, J. (2016). Licensing Internal Arguments. In: Building Predicates. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol 92. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28682-2_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28682-2_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-28680-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-28682-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics