Keywords

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Background

The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster displays rest-activity rhythms that rely on a circadian clock located in the brain. In light–dark (LD) cycles, adult flies show a bimodal activity with morning and evening peaks at dawn and dusk. Activity rhythms persist in constant darkness (DD), indicating the circadian nature of this behavior. Like peripheral clocks, the brain clock depends on a molecular feedback loop where the CLOCK (CLK) and CYCLE (CYC) transcriptional factors drive the expression of the PERIOD (PER) and TIMELESS (TIM) proteins that repress CLK/CYC activity. The negative feedback loop operates in about 150 neurons, the so-called clock neurons, which account for about 0.1 % of the total CNS neural population. The numerical simplicity of these 150 neurons that form a network is remarkable in comparison to central circuits for other hardwired behaviors such as courtship or learning and memory. The Drosophila clock neuronal network is also remarkably simple in comparison to circadian control circuits in vertebrates, where several brain areas, including the hypothalamus, pituitary gland, pineal gland, olfactory bulb, etc., harbor numerous bona fide clock neurons. This smaller number of neurons offers the potential to manipulate oscillators at the single-cell level in vivo, through well-defined genetic handles.

There are two broad populations within the 150 clock neurons of the fly brain; one population is laterally placed and another is located along the dorsal margin of the brain. The lateral neurons (LNs) lie near the interface of the central brain and the optic lobe and are organized into a ventral cluster that include small (s-LNvs) and large (l-LNvs) cells, a dorsal cluster (LNds) and a posterior cluster (LPNs). The dorsal neurons (DNs) are in turn subdivided into three clusters designated as DN1, DN2 and DN3 (Fig. 1). Such anatomical categorization frequently has neurochemical and functional bases; for example, the four most ventral s-LNvs express the Pigment-dispersing factor (PDF) neuropeptide and promote morning activity in LD. Based on strong functional data, mostly behavioral and some neurophysiological in nature, a wiring diagram of these differentiated clusters of brain clock neurons has begun to materialize over the past 10 years. In the following section we will summarize the logic of organization of this circuit.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Left panel: seven groups of clock neurons have been defined on an anatomical basis. The lateral neurons are organized into a ventral cluster that include small (s-LNvs) and large (l-LNvs) cells, a dorsal cluster (LNds) and a posterior cluster (LPNs). The dorsal neurons include three clusters designated as DN1, DN2 and DN3. Several neuropiles are indicated: Medulla (ME) and Lobula (LOB) in the optic lobe and Pars Intercerebralis (PI), Mushroom Bodies (MB) and Ellipsoid Body (EB) in the central brain. Central panel: projections of the different clock neuron subsets: s-LNvs and l-LNvs (orange), LNds and fifth PDF-negative s-LNv (red), DN1s (blue). Right panel: communication between neuronal clusters involves PDF from s-LNvs to LNds and DN1s as well as from l-LNvs to LNds, and glutamate from DN1s to s-LNvs

Layout of the Network in Constant Conditions

In the absence of cycling environmental cues, the fly clock circuit has been shown to adopt a functionally minimalist organization. In flies that experienced light–dark cycles and were subsequently kept in constant darkness, the presence of PDF-expressing s-LNvs was required to drive robust rhythmic behavior (Helfrich-Förster 1998; Renn et al. 1999) and a clock restricted to the PDF-expressing cells was sufficient to drive 24-h rhythms (Grima et al. 2004). In contrast, PDF-negative neurons drove behavioral rhythmicity under constant light if light inputs were reduced by the absence of the cell-resident photopigment cryptochrome (CRY). The precise location of these neurons that act as the pacemaker in constant light (LL) has been suggested to be either within the LNd cluster (Picot et al. 2007) or within the DN1s (Murad et al. 2007; Stoleru et al. 2007). In spite of running a functioning oscillator, the s-LNv neurons fail to influence the behavioral period in LL (Picot et al. 2007; Stoleru et al. 2007). The predominant contribution of the s-LNvs to behavioral rhythmicity, as evident in DD, becomes dramatically corroded in LL as ambient light inhibits their behavioral output (Picot et al. 2007). Under constant conditions the operation of the clock circuit remains highly centralized, but depending on the sustained presence or absence of light, this central position is occupied by either the PDF-negative clock neurons or the PDF-positive s-LNv neurons, respectively. Notably, in DD, the s-LNvs operate at the pinnacle of a hierarchy as they enforce a majority of other oscillators to realign their clock program in accordance with the sLNv pace (Stoleru et al. 2005). In contrast, messages from the non-PDF clocks have considerably subdued influence on the running of the master pacemaker in DD (Stoleru et al. 2005; Picot et al. 2007; Collins et al. 2014).

Recently, however, the existence of a centralized monopolar circuit organization in DD has been seriously challenged. The PDF clock has been shown to coherently change behavioral period only over a limited range, which is distributed asymmetrically around the 24-h focal point (Yao and Shafer 2014; Beckwith and Ceriani 2015). When the PDF neurons were forced to run at a pace beyond this specified range, multiple peaks of behavioral period emerged within a single fly, likely as a result of internal desynchronization among multiple oscillators (Yao and Shafer 2014; Beckwith and Ceriani 2015). Because these oscillators are coupled to the PDF clock with differing strength and range of entrainment, they are differentially affected by speed changes in the PDF clock (Yao and Shafer 2014). Thus, the behavioral period in DD is determined by the pace of not only the s-LNv clock but also by other oscillators enjoying different degrees of independence, although they were formerly thought to uniformly behave as slaves of the s-LNv pacemaker. As opposed to direct manipulation of individual oscillator pace, a parallel line of research was to putatively increase the excitability of different subsets of clock neurons to enhance their contribution in the network. This study raised the interesting possibility that the CRY-negative clock neurons, e.g., the DN2s, may have the potential to affect behavioral period like the well-known s-LNvs (Dissel et al. 2014). Going one step further, it was proposed that the DD behavioral period is constructed by integrating the slightly longer period dictated by the s-LNvs and the slightly shorter period imposed by the DN2s, with other clock neurons modulating the contributions of these two oscillators (Dissel et al. 2014). The behavioral period would thus depend on the interactions between differently paced oscillators whose endogenous period and influence in the network vary according to environmental conditions. However, speed changes in all clock neurons excluding the PDF cells fell short of altering the behavioral period (Yao and Shafer 2014), in fact bolstering the older idea that PDF oscillators are the predominant determinant of the behavior period in DD. In absence of PDF signaling, the output from the s-LNvs was compromised, thereby allowing secondary oscillators to strongly influence the behavior period (Yao and Shafer 2014). Precisely which oscillators are coupled, whether coupling is directional, how the coupling strength is determined and what are the relative weights of different oscillators to behavioral period according to environmental conditions are some of the questions that fly chronobiologists will probably resolve in next few years. We predict that the existing momentum on neuronal mechanisms of behavioral period determination will be extended to understand the other fundamental parameters of rhythm, such as phase and waveform.

Network Architecture Under LD Cycles

Depending on the constraints of physiological thermal limit and light availability, animals evolved few basic patterns of diel activity: diurnal, nocturnal, crepuscular or cathemeral (Bennie et al. 2014). In mammals, a given animal can stably and predictably switch back and forth between different patterns in context-dependent ways (Kas and Edgar 1999; Mrosovsky 2003). The choice of a temporal niche takes place downstream of the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) clock and is strongly influenced by light inputs (Mrosovsky and Hattar 2005; Doyle et al. 2008). A comparable plasticity is observed in flies. For example, a typically crepuscular male fly will become nocturnal in the presence of a mate or during moonlit nights (Bachleitner et al. 2007; Fujii et al. 2007) and will be more diurnal when daylight is low (Schlichting et al. 2015). At first glance, the similar phasing of molecular oscillations in the different clock neuron subsets of the brain suggests that shaping the sleep-wake cycle occurs downstream of the clock, but results obtained from manipulating these different subsets support a more complex model.

Flies in the standard laboratory condition of 12:12 LD cycles show a bimodal profile with peaks of activity coinciding with the putative twilight transitions. Very nicely, the behavioral sub-routines of generating a morning peak and an evening peak are orchestrated by two separable subsets of oscillator neurons, the s-LNvs and the LNds, respectively, providing concrete experimental support for the dual-oscillator model of Daan and Pittendrigh (Pittendrigh and Daan 1976; Grima et al. 2004; Stoleru et al. 2004) (Fig. 2). Of note, this dual-oscillator ground plan contrasts with the predominantly monopolar hierarchical organization prevailing in DD. The rather autonomous operation of two oscillators under LD cycles is abetted by their independent access to light information through redundant pathways—in-house CRY signaling and the visual system—whose output impinges on the clock circuit probably at multiple nodes, including the LNvs (Cusumano et al. 2009; Zhang et al. 2009). However, network interaction between oscillator neuron clusters could still be evident in LD. In the absence of PDF secretion by the LNvs, the evening peak of activity is advanced by a couple of hours (Renn et al. 1999). In the absence of both CRY and PDF, the evening peak vanishes and the phase of the molecular oscillations in the LNds is strongly altered (Cusumano et al. 2009; Zhang et al. 2009; Im et al. 2011). The phasing of the evening activity thus increasingly depends on PDF signaling when autonomous CRY-dependent photoreception decreases at the end of the day because of less intense and more reddish light. So far, the role of DNs in shaping the LD activity pattern seems to be largely secondary to the LNs (Grima et al. 2004; Stoleru et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2010a, b). A functional clock restricted to the DN1s is sufficient to drive both morning and evening activity bouts in low light LD conditions, whereas high light permits morning activity only (Zhang et al. 2010b). These outputs are affected by temperature, and the DN1 neurons thus appear to be capable of integrating certain light and temperature information from the ambient environment (Zhang et al. 2010a, b). Although the expression of PDFR in the DN1s is important for their proper function (Zhang et al. 2010a), how they modulate the clock network’s collective output remains unknown.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Contribution of different clock neuron subsets to the LD behavior. Activity plots show the contribution of neuronal groups to morning and evening LD behavior. s-LNvs promote morning activity (left) whereas LNds and the fifth PDF-negative s-LNv promote evening activity (center). DN1s can promote morning activity and evening activity (right). The evening output of the DN1s is very weak in high light but strongly increases in low light (dashed line)

In summary, under periodic environmental cues, multiple, highly autonomous oscillators with distinct behavioral contributions collaboratively sculpt the organism’s activity profile. In line with the ‘internal coincidence model’ of photoperiodism (Pittendrigh and Minis 1964), flexible changes in the clock network favoring the contribution of particular oscillators under certain ambient environmental conditions have been put forward as the mechanistic basis of seasonal adaptation in flies (Stoleru et al. 2007). The Daan/Pittendrigh model proposed that light accelerates morning oscillators and decelerates evening oscillators to adapt the bimodal activity to the changing photoperiod. Fast- and slow-running neuronal oscillators were described in flies displaying split rhythms in LL (Yoshii et al. 2004). However, short and long period components were observed to derive from the LD evening bout, suggesting that light-accelerated clock neurons contribute to the evening activity (Yoshii et al. 2004; Rieger et al. 2006), in contrast to the prediction of the model. As indicated above, light was shown to promote the output of the LNd-based evening oscillator while inhibiting the morning oscillator carried by the PDF-expressing s-LNvs (Picot et al. 2007). The importance of PDF-negative cells in the presence of light is also shown by experiments comparing the relative influence of PDF-positive and PDF-negative neuronal subsets in different photoperiods. This work was done by looking at morning and evening activity peaks of flies with accelerated PDF-positive or PDF-negative neurons. Under long photoperiods, the evening oscillator located in PDF-negative cells was proposed to control the speed of the morning oscillators, whereas in short photoperiod conditions the morning oscillator of PDF cells would take the lead (Stoleru et al. 2007). The discovery of other subsets contributing to morning and evening activity bouts, in particular the DN1s contributing to evening activity in low light only, suggest that the adaptation to photoperiod changes might be more complex. Indeed, we have data indicating that new groups of oscillators are recruited when flies are confronted with summer-like conditions. Such laboratory-based simplified environmental parameters are probably inadequate to explain the working of the network under the complex natural conditions that exist in the spatiotemporal niche inhabited by Drosophila in the wild (Menegazzi et al. 2012, 2013; Vanin et al. 2012; De et al. 2013). In particular, daily temperature variations have a strong impact on the sleep-activity pattern and can even induce some morning and evening anticipatory activity in clockless flies (Vanin et al. 2012; Menegazzi et al. 2013). But the principles and logic of circuit operation learned from a severely artificial set-up could well be valid and applicable for the same network’s functionality under more complex natural conditions.