Advertisement

Perception, Action and the Notion of Grounding

  • Alexandros TillasEmail author
  • Gottfried Vosgerau
Chapter
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 376)

Abstract

Traditionally, philosophers and cognitive scientists alike considered the mind as divided into input units (perception), central processing (cognition), and output units (action). In turn, they allowed for little – if any – direct interaction between perception and action. In recent years, theorists challenged the classical view of the mind by arguing that bodily states ground cognition. Even though promising, the notion of grounding is largely underspecified. In this paper, we focus on the debate about the relation between perception and action in order to flesh out the process and in turn clarify the notion of grounding. Given that currently the debate about the relation between perception & action is far from settled, we attempt an assessment of the implications that possible outcomes of this debate would have on Grounding Cognition Theories. Interestingly, some of these possible outcomes seem to threaten the overall program of Grounded Cognition. In an attempt to make this analysis more concrete, we study two closely related speculative hypotheses about possible ways in which perception and action interact. Namely, we focus on Theory of Event Coding and Simulation Theory, and evaluate the levels of compatibility between those two views and Grounded Cognition Theories.

Keywords

Perception Action Embodiment Grounding Grounded cognition 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Patrice Soom, James Trafford, and Uwe Peters for their help and comments on earlier drafts.

References

  1. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–609.Google Scholar
  2. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brandt, S. A., & Stark, L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17, 1527–1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 913–919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Demarais, A. M., & Cohen, B. H. (1998). Evidence for image-scanning eye movements during transitive inference. Biological Psychology, 49(3), 229–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Farah, M. J. (1989). The neuropsychology of mental imagery. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 2, pp. 239–248). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  9. Farah, M. J. (1995). Current issues in the neuropsychology of image generation. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1455–1471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Findlay, J. M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2003). Active vision: The psychology of looking and seeing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Finke, R. A. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  12. Gallese, V. (2007). The ‘conscious’ dorsal stream: Embodied simulation and its role in space and action conscious awareness. Psyche, 13(1) (archived electronic journal: http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/)
  13. Gangopadhyay, N., Madary, M., & Spicer, F. (2010). Perception, action and consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (2004). Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Horst, S. (2009). The computational theory of mind. In Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy entry. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/computational-mind/
  17. Hurley, S. (2001). Perception and action: Alternative views. Synthese, 129, 3–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jacob, P., & de Vignemont, F. (2010). Spatial coordinates and phenomenology in the two-visual systems model. In N. Gangopadhyay, M. Madary, & F. Spicer (Eds.), Perception, action and consciousness (pp. 125–144). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2003). Ways of seeing, the scope and limits of visual cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kiverstein, J. (2010). Sensorimotor knowledge and the contents of experience. In N. Gangopdhay, M. Madary, & F. Spicer (Eds.), Perception, action and consciousness: Sensorimotor dynamics and dual vision (pp. 257–275). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Kim, I. J., & Alpert, N. M. (1995). Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature, 378, 496–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2010). Cortical visual systems for perception and action. In N. Gangopadhyay, M. Madary, & F. Spicer (Eds.), Perception, action and consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Noë, A. (2005). Action in perception (pp. 71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. Norton, D., & Stark, L. W. (1971). Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vision Research, 11, 929–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. O’Regan, K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 883–917.Google Scholar
  27. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  29. Shapiro, L. (2007). The embodied cognition research programme. Philosophy Compass. Article first published online 2 feb 2007. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-9991.2007.00064.x.
  30. Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 65(4), 235–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Tillas, A. (2010). Back to out senses: An empiricist on concept acquisition. Ph.D. thesis. University of Bristol.Google Scholar
  32. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24(3), 830–846.Google Scholar
  33. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116, 185–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. W. A. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  35. Weber, A., & Vosgerau, G. (2012). Grounding action representations. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3, 53–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Contextual processing of abstract concepts reveals neural representations of nonlinguistic semantic content. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(6), 920–935. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of DüsseldorfDüsseldorfGermany

Personalised recommendations