Advertisement

Clark Lecture in 1968 “Processes of Cognitive Growth: Infancy”

  • Jerome S. BrunerEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Cultural Psychology of Education book series (CPED, volume 2)

Abstract

Distinguished colleagues, ladies and gentlemen. I am deeply honored and personally moved to be the Heinz Werner Lecturer. For Heinz Werner was a man whose stature and purpose give dignity and substance to the study of development.

Keywords

Voluntary Control Precision Grip Primate Evolution Power Grip Human Skill 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Alt, J. (1968). The use of vision in early reaching. Unpublished honors thesis, Department of Psychology, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  3. Ashby, W. R. (1952). Design fora Brain. London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Balint, M. (1948). Individual differences of behavior in early infancy, and an objective method for recording them. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 73, 53–117.Google Scholar
  5. Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movement. New York: Peigamon Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bever, T. G. (1961). Pre-linguistic behavior. Unpublished honors thesis, Department of Linguistics, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  7. Bower, T. G. R. (1966). The visual world of infants. Scientific American, 215(6), 80–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brackbill, Y. (1967). The use of social reinforcement in conditioning smiling. In Y. Brackbill & G. G. Thompson (Eds.), Behavior in infancy and early childhood (pp. 616–625). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bridger, W. H. (1962). Ethological concepts and human development. Recent Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 4, 95–107.Google Scholar
  10. Bronshtein, A. I., & Petrova, E. P. (1967). The auditory analyzer in young infants. In Y. Brackbill & G. G. Thompson (Eds.), Behavior in infancy and early childhood (pp. 163–172). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. M. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  12. Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 6, 345–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Buettner-Janusch, J. (1966). Origins of man. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Burkitt, M. (1963). The old slone age. New York: Atheneum.Google Scholar
  15. Chomsky, N. (1967). Recent contributions to the theory of innate ideas. Synthese, 17, 2–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Conel, J. L. (1939–1963). The postnatal development of the human cerebral cortex (Vol. I–Vl). Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  17. David, M. (1967). Report to the Ciba Conference on Infancy, London.Google Scholar
  18. de Laguna, G. (1927). Speech: its function and development. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press, 1963. (First published, 1927).Google Scholar
  19. Freedman, D. G., Loring, C. B., & Martin, R. M. (1967). Emotional behavior and personality development. In Y. Brackbill (Ed.), Infancy and early childhood (pp. 429–502). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  20. Goodman, N. (1967). The epistemological argument. Synthese, 17(23), 28.Google Scholar
  21. Gregory, R. L. (1966). Eye and brain; the psychology of seeing. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  22. Gunther, M. (1961). Infant behavior at the breast. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 37–44, Vol. 1). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  23. Halverson, H. M. (1931). An experimental study of prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema records. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 10, 107–286.Google Scholar
  24. Held, R. (1965). Plasticity in sensory-motor systems. Scientific American, 213(5), 84–94.Google Scholar
  25. Irwin, O. C. (1947). Infant speech: Consonantal sounds according to place of articulation. Journal of Speech Hearing Disorders, 12, 397–401.Google Scholar
  26. Irwin, O. C. (1948). Infant speech: Development of vowel sounds. Journal of Speech Hearing Disorders, 13, 31–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, aphasie und allgemeine leutgesetze. Uppsala: Almgrist and Weksell.Google Scholar
  28. Jensen, K. (1932). Differential reactions to taste and temperature stimuli in newborn infants. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 12, 361–479.Google Scholar
  29. Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur behavior: A madagascar field study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  30. Jonckheere, A. R. (1967). Seventh Annual Report of the Center for Cognitive Studies, Harvard University. Google Scholar
  31. Katz, J. J. (1966). The philosophy of language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  32. Kessen, W. (1967). Sucking and looking: two organized congenital patterns of behavior in the human newborn. In H. W. Stevenson, E. H. Hess & H. L. Rheingold (Eds.), Early behavior comparative anil developmental approaches (pp. 147–179). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  33. Kessen, W., & Leutzendorff, A. M. (1963). The effect of nonnutritive sucking on movement in the human newborn. Journal of Comparative and physiological Psychology, 56, 69–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Klüver, H. (1933). Behavior mechanisms in monkeys. Chicago: University of Chicago Press 1960. (Originally published, 1933).Google Scholar
  35. Knapp, H. D., Taub, E., & Berman, A. J. (1963). Movements in monkeys with deafferented forelimbs. Experimental Neurology, 7(4), 305–315.Google Scholar
  36. Kron, R. E., Stein, M., & Goddard, K. E. (1963). A method of measuring sucking behavior of newborn infants. Psychosomatic Medicine, 25, 181–191.Google Scholar
  37. Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon Symposium (pp. 112–146). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Lassek, A. M. (1948). The pyramidal tract: Basic considerations of corticospinal neurons. In The Frontal Lobes. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease (pp. 106–128, Vol. XXVII). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
  39. Latif, I. (1934). The physiological basis of linguistic development and of the ontogeny of meaning: I, II. Psychological Review, 41(55–85), 153–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. LeGros Clark, W. E. (1963). The antecedents of man. New York: Harper & Row. (First published, 1959).Google Scholar
  41. Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  42. Lévi-Strauss, C. (1963). Structural anthropology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  43. Lipsitt, L. P., & Kaye, H. (1964). Conditioned sucking in the human newborn. Psychonomic Science, 1, 29–30.Google Scholar
  44. Luria, V. R. (1966). Human brain and psychological processes. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  45. MacKav, D. M. (1966). Conscious control of action. In J. C. Eccles (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience (pp. 422–445). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  46. McGraw, M. B. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  47. McNeill, D. (1966). Developmental psycholinguistics. In F. Smith & G. A. Miller (Eds.), The genesis of language: A psycholinguistic approach (pp. 15–91). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  48. McNeill, D. The development of language. In P. A. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology. New York: Wiley (in press).Google Scholar
  49. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Henry Holt & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mott, F. W., & Sherrington, C. S. (1895). Experiments on the influence of sensory nerves upon movement and nutrition of the limbs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 57, 481–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
  53. Oakley, K. P. (1960). Man the toolmaker. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  54. Peiper, A. (1963). Cerebral function hi infancy and childhood. New York: Consultants Bureau.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Piaget, J. (1968). On the development of memory and identity: Heinz Werner lecture. Worcester: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, Barbel. (1966). L’image mentale chez l’enfant; étude sur le développement des representations imagées. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  59. Rheingold, H. L., Gewirtz, J. L., & Ross, H. W. (1959). Social conditioning of vocalizations in the infant. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 68–73.Google Scholar
  60. Richardson, H. M. (1932). The growth of adaptive behavior in infants: An experimental study at seven age levels. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 12(3–4), 195–359.Google Scholar
  61. Robson, K. S. (1967). The role of eye-to-eye contact in maternal-infant attachment. Bethesda: Child Research Branch, NIMH, Bethesda.Google Scholar
  62. Rowell, T. E., & Hinde, R. A. (1962). Vocal communication by the Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatto). Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 138, 279–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Rylander, G. (1948). Personality analysis before and after frontal lobotomy. In The Frontal Lobes. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease (pp. 691–705, Vol. XXVII). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
  64. Sameroff, A. J. (1965). An experimental study of the response components of sucking in the human newborn. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
  65. Sander, L. (1962). Issues in early mother-child interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1(1), 141–166.Google Scholar
  66. Sander, L. (1964). Adaptive relationships in early mother-child interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 3(2), 231–264.Google Scholar
  67. Sander, L. (1968). Personal communication.Google Scholar
  68. Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, Illinois: Row, Peterson.Google Scholar
  69. Siqueland, E. R. (1968). Conditioned sucking and visual reinforcers with human infants. Paper presented at Eastern Regional meeting, Society for Research in Child Development, Worcester, Mass.Google Scholar
  70. Sokolov, Y. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  71. Teuber, H.-L. (1966). Alterations of perception after brain injury. In J. C. Eccles (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience (pp. 182–216). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  72. Trevarthen, C. B. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung, 31, 229–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Trevarthen, C. B., & Richards, M. Development of communication and object-directed behavior in early infancy. Unpublished study, Center for Cognitive Studies, Harvard University, (in preparation).Google Scholar
  74. Twitchell, T. E. (1954). Sensory factors in purposive movement. Journal of Neurophysiology, 17, 239–252.Google Scholar
  75. Twitchell, T. E. (1965). The automatic grasping responses of infants. Neuropsychologin, 3, 247–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. von Holst, E., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip. Natunvissenschaften, 37, 464–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Washburn, Margaret F. (1916). Movement and mental imagery. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
  78. White, B. L., Castle, P., & Held, R. (1964). Observations on the development of visually-directed reaching. Child Development, 35, 349–364.Google Scholar
  79. Wolff, P. H. (1968). Personal communication.Google Scholar
  80. Wolff, P. H., & Simmons, M. A. (1967). Nonnutritive sucking and response thresholds in young infants. Child Development, 38(3), 631–638.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Director of the Center for Cognitive StudiesHarvard UniversityCambridgeUSA

Personalised recommendations