Skip to main content

Socialization as Biological-Social Interaction

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Autonomous Child

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Well-Being and Quality of Life Research ((BRIEFSWELLBEING))

  • 1344 Accesses

Abstract

Socialization is a social process with strong biological components that interact with social and cultural frameworks and factors. The chapter illustrates the interplay of social and genetic factors, referring to studies that underline that babies are biologically designed for moving into communities of signs, as well as to meta-perspectives on the relationship between evolution and culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    See Sahlins: http://www.eco-action.org/dt/affluent.html (retreived January 4, 2015).

  2. 2.

    See: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnins.2014.00276/full.

  3. 3.

    http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0016006.

References

  • Bateson, M. C. (1979). The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 63–77). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Houts, R. M., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2010). Infant attachment security and the timing of puberty: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 21(9), 1195–1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnebekk, G. (2007). Dispositions related to sensitivity in the neurological basis for activation of approach-avoidance motivation, antisocial attributes and individual differences in aggressive behavior. Social Behaviorist and Personality, 35(9), 1251–1264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, S. (1998). Infant learning by altercentric participation: The reverse of egocentric observation in autism. In S. Bråten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, S. (2007). Altercentric infants and adults: On the origins and manifestations of participant perception of others’ acts and utterances. In S. Bråten (Ed.), On being moved. From mirror neurons to empathy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, S. (2009). The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brook, A., & Ross, D. (2002). Daniel dennett. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., Jolly, A., & Sylva, K. (Eds.). (1976). Play: Its role in development and evolution. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, A. (Ed.). (1978). The sociobiology debate: Readings on ethical and scientific issues. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2006). Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 583–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., Wright, B. R. R., Moffitt, T. R., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Early failure in the labor market: Childhood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition to adulthood. American Sociological Review, 63, 424–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conley, D., Strully, K. W., & Bennett, N. G. (2003). The starting gate: Birth weight and life chances. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (2006). The god delusion. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I. J., Taylor, M. D., Hart, C. L., Wilson, V., Smith, G. D., Blane, D., & Starr, J. M. (2005). Intergenerational social mobility and mid-life status attainment: Influences of childhood intelligence, childhood social factors, and education. Intelligence, 33(5), 455–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (2004). Freedom evolves. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. (2008). Practice and public policy in the area of gene-environment interactions. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Genetic effects on environmental vulnerability and disease (pp. 87–97). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinstein, L. (2003). Inequality in the early cognitive development of British children in the 1970 cohort. Economica, 70(277), 73–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frønes, I. (1995). Among peers. Oslo, Stockholm: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frønes, I. (2007). On theories of dialogue, self and society: Redefining socialization and the acquisition of meaning in light of the intersubjective matrix. In S. Bråten (Ed.), On being moved. From mirror neurons to empathy (pp. 201–217). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottesman, I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groos, K. (1898) The play of animals (New York 1898) https://archive.org/details/playanimals00baldgoog.

  • Habermas, J. (2007). The language game of responsible agency and the problem of free will: How can epistemic dualism be reconciled with ontological monism? Philosophical Explorations, 10(1), 13–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, J. L., Hair, N., Shen, D. G., Shi, F., Gilmore, J. H., Wolfe, B. L., et al. (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLoS ONE, 8, e80954. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080954 http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0080954#references. Accessed July 2, 2015.

  • Harlow, H. F. (1962). Affectional systems of monkeys, involving relations between mothers and young. In International Symposium on Comparative Medicine Proceedings (pp. 6–10). New York: Eaton Laboratories.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haworth, C. M. A., Asbury, K., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2011). Added value measures in education show genetic as well as environmental influence. PLoS ONE, 6(2), e16006. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M., Reiss, D., & Plomin, R. (1994). Separate social worlds of siblings: The impact of non-shared environment on development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaler, S. R., & Freeman, B. J. (1994). Analysis of environmental deprivation: Cognitive and social development in Romanian orphans. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(4), 769–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry, L. M., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities Affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 1106–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, M., Barnes, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Self-referential cognition and empathy in autism. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000883 http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0000883. Accessed July 2, 2015.

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children’s mental health: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11(10), 903–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montessori, M. (1949). The absorbent mind. Free download https://archive.org/details/absorbentmind031961mbp. Accessed July 2, 2015.

  • Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from each other? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 122–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Mehta, M., et al. (2007). Effects of profound early institutional deprivation: An overview of findings from a UK longitudinal study of Romanian adoptees. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 332–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. (2004). Stone age economics. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberman, S. (2001). The geek syndrome. Wired. Issue 9.12 http://archive.wired.com/wired/archive/9.12/aspergers.html. Accessed July 2, 2015.

  • Tiger, L., & Fox, R. (1978). The human biogram. In A. L. Caplan (Ed.), The sociobiology debate (pp. 57–63). New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. B. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before Speech (Ed.), Bullowa, M (pp. 321–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. B. (2011). What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being Infant and child development special issue: The intersubjective newborn, 20(1), 119–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer, E., Haley, A. P., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. C. (2000). Non-shared environment: A theoretical, methodological and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 78–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2010). The evolution of Harry Harlow: From the nature to the nurture of love. History of Psychiatry, 21(2), 1–16 http://individual.utoronto.ca/vicedo/vicedoca/Publications_files/Vicedo_HofP.pdf. Accessed July 2, 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Stumm, S., Macintyre, S., Batty, D. G., Clark, H., & Deary, I. J. (2010). Intelligence, social class of origin, childhood behavior disturbance and education as predictors of status attainment in midlife in men: The Aberdeen Children of the 1950s study. Intelligence, 38(1), 202–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldfogel, J., & Washbrook, E. (2010). Low income and early cognitive development in the U.K. A Report for the Sutton Trust February 2010. http://www.suttontrust.com/wp-content/uploads/2010/02/Sutton_Trust_Cognitive_Report.pdf.

  • Wilson, E. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. (1978). The biology of sociobiology. In Caplan. A. L. (Ed.), The sociobiology debate. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ivar Frønes .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Frønes, I. (2016). Socialization as Biological-Social Interaction. In: The Autonomous Child. SpringerBriefs in Well-Being and Quality of Life Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-25100-4_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-25100-4_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-25098-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-25100-4

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics