Advertisement

Gender Differences in Temporal Data Analysis

Toward Women or Men Empowerment?
  • Ilona NawrotEmail author
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 9187)

Abstract

The evidence shows that time-oriented data visualizations may greatly benefit from time spatialization (organization of time along axes) adaptation. Specifically, the choice of time arrangement along axes can significantly influence performance and satisfaction of such visualizations’ users. However, due to gender differences in visuospatial abilities and approaches to solve spatial problems (how males and females navigate through real or imaginary space), males and females may respond differently to time spatialization adaptations. This in turn can generate demand for gender-specific interventions or system solutions. Approaching this important theoretical and practical issue, here, we report the results of the experiment that investigated the problem of gender differences in the productivity of interactions with time-oriented data visualizations. We found no sufficient evidence to confirm the existence of such differences. We yet recommend further research in this area to completely eliminate this possibility.

Keywords

Timeline adaptation Time perception Time spatialization Gender differences 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work has been supported by The French Ministry of Foreign Affairs through the Eiffel Excellence Scholarship.

References

  1. 1.
    Alhola, P., Polo-Kantola, P.: Sleep deprivation: impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatric Dis. Treat. 3(5), 553–567 (2007)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Barsalou, L.W.: Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Boroditsky, L.: Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cogn. Psychol. 43(1), 1–22 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Boroditsky, L.: How languages construct time. In: Dehaene, S., Brannon, E. (eds.) Space, Time and Number in the Brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought, pp. 333–341. Academic Press, London (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K.: Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition 118(1), 123–129 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Devore, E., Grodstein, F., Duffy, J., Stampfer, M., Czeisler, C., Schernhammer, E.: Sleep duration in midlife and later life in relation to cognition. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62(6), 1073–1081 (2014)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Fuhrman, O., Boroditsky, L.: Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time: evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cogn. Sci. 34(8), 1430–1451 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K., Chen, E., Jiang, H., Shu, D., Mao, S., Boroditsky, L.: How linguistic and cultural forces shape conceptions of time: English and Mandarin time in 3D. Cogn. Sci. 35(7), 1305–1328 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Halpern, D.F.: Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities, 4th edn. Psychology Press, New York (2012)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Inzlicht, M., Tullett, A.M., Legault, L.: Lingering effects: stereotype threat hurts more than you think. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 5(1), 227–256 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Kessell, A., Tversky, B.: Visualizing space, time, and agents: production, performance, and preference. Cogn. Process. 12(1), 43–52 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Kessell, A.M.: Cognitive methods for information visualization: linear and cyclical events. Ph.D. Dissertation (2008). https://www.stanford.edu/dept/psychology/cgi-bin/drupalm/system/files/Kessell_Dissertation.08.pdf
  13. 13.
    Killgore, W.D.S., Weber, M.: Sleep deprivation and cognitive performance. In: Bianchi, M.T. (ed.) Sleep Deprivation and Disease: Effects on the Body, Brain and Behavior, pp. 209–229. Springer, New York (2014)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Komarov, S., Reinecke, K., Gajos, K.Z.: Crowdsourcing performance evaluations of user interfaces. In: Bødker, S., Brewster, S., Baudisch, P., Beaudouin-Lafon, M., Mackay, W.E. (eds.) Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 207–216. ACM, New York (2013)Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Lakoff, G., Johnson, M.: Metaphors We Live By. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Lakoff, G., Johnson, M.: Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. J. Philos. 77(8), 453–486 (1980)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    McGlone, M.S., Aronson, J.: Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 27(1), 486–493 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Miles, L.K., Tan, L., Noble, G.D., Lumsden, J., Macrae, C.N.: Can a mind have two time lines? Exploring space-time mapping in Mandarin and English speakers. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18(3), 598–604 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Moè, A.: Gender differences does not mean genetic difference: externalizng improves performance in mental rotation. Lear. Individ. Differ. 22(1), 20–24 (2012)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Nawrot, I., Doucet, A.: Timeline localization. In: Kurosu, M. (ed.) HCI 2014, Part I. LNCS, vol. 8510, pp. 611–622. Springer, Heidelberg (2014) Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Nunez, R.E., Sweetser, E.: With the future behind them: convergent evidence from aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cogn. Sci. 30(3), 401–450 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., Gabay, S.: Is the future the right time? Exp. Psychol. 57(4), 308–314 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Reinecke, K., Bernstein, A.: Improving performance, perceived usability, and aesthetics with culturally adaptive user interfaces. ToCHI 18(2), A:1–A:29 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Santiago, J., Román, A., Ouellet, M.: Flexible foundations of abstract thought: a review and a theory. In: Maass, A., Schubert, T. (eds.) Spatial dimensions of social thought, pp. 39–108. De Gruyter, Berlin (2011)Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Sauro, J., Lewis, J.R.: Average task times in usability tests: what to report? In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 2347–2350. ACM, Atlanta (2010)Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Schmader, T., Johns, M.: Converging evedence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85(3), 440–452 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., Winter, A.: Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cogn. Psychol. 23(4), 515–557 (1991)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Wendt, H.W.: Dealing with a common problem in social science: a simplified rank-biserial coefficient of correlation based on the \(U\) statistic. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2(4), 463–465 (1972)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Université de La RochelleLa RochelleFrance
  2. 2.Poznań University of Economics, WIGE, KEMPoznańPoland

Personalised recommendations