Skip to main content

Concluding Remarks

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Construction Learning as a Complex Adaptive System

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Education ((BRIEFSEDUCAT))

  • 751 Accesses

Abstract

This final chapter outlines some conclusive observations and discusses further the import of constructional priming as a clear example of language as a complex adaptive system. In any system, agents and processes interact at and across layers of its organization, thus giving rise to unpredictable outcomes. In their complex and erratic interactional behaviours, the agents prompt the whole system to go through spontaneous self-organization, which leads to emergent patterned outputs. In turn, the system, being itself part of a broader environment, adapts to the changes of the environment, and, as a result of the novel adaptive behaviour of the system, the environment evolves in a never-ending cycle of changes. Likewise, language is a complex adaptive system where the speakers’ joint discursive actions remould its internal configuration in dynamic and unpredictable ways into a by-product of communication emerging from competing social, cognitive and physical factors. Language is a meaning-making instrument that emerges from the constant interplay between the discursive context, the physical body and the mind within the complex dynamic system of enaction, a form of coupling where the agents and the environment are not separated but they mutually influence and determine each other. Meaning construction is motivated by well-rooted and recurring patterns of embodied experience in the form of image-schematic structures, and neuroscience research has proved that it involves partial re-enactment of the sensori-motor states. Hence language patterns are entrenched neuro-motor routines following from the high-frequent usage in communicative events: repetition of linguistic expressions produce bio-chemical mental traces that engender entrenched behaviours, which recent usage-based theories of grammar ascribe to the ontological status of constructions that, being systematically associated with unitary mental representations, consist of nodes of entrenched patterns along degrees of complexity and schematicity in the linguistic system. In the light of recent research results in neurolinguistics and banking on evidence from psycholinguistic experiments with priming methodologies, language acquisition can be equated to the learning of constructions not only in L1 speakers but also in L2 learners.

If the units of language are constructions, then language acquisition is the learning of constructions.

Ellis (2013, p. 368)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 173–218). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bencini, G., & Goldberg, A. (2000). The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 640–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R., & Pickering, M. (2007). Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 931–949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, H. (2009). Verb meanings at the crossroads between higher-level and lower-level constructions. Lingua, 120, 22–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In M.A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H., Pickering, M., Liversedge, S., Stewart, A., & Urbach, T. (1995). Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 489–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, C. (2003). Structure and function. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., Dell, G., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H., & Clark, E. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • ColomĂ©, A. (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals’ speech production: Language-specific or language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 721–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijkstra, T., Van Heuven, W., & Grainger, J. (1998). Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 177–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (2006). Cognitive perspectives on SLA: The associative cognitive CREED. AILA Review, 19, 100–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. C. (2013). Second language acquisition. In G. Trousdale & T. Hoffmann (Eds.), Oxford handbook of construction grammar (pp. 365–378). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. (2004). An alternative view of the mental lexicon. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 301–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1979). On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut. West Bend, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1982). Making and Correcting Errors during Sentence Comprehension: Eye Movements in the Analysis of Structurally Ambiguous Sentences.  Cognition, 14, 178–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A., Shillcock, R., Brown, G., Mill, A., & Cutler, A. (1982). Slips of the tongue in the London-Lund corpus of spontaneous conversation. In A. Cutler (Ed.), Slips of the tongue and language production (pp. 251–263). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. (1975). The analysis of sentence production. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 133–177). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. (1988). Processes in language production. In Frederick Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey (vol. 3). Language: Psychological and biological aspects, (pp. 69–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • GivĂłn, T. (1984). Syntax: A functional-typological introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (1999). The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Emergence of language (pp. 197–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F., Grosjean, L., & Lane, H. (1979). The patterns of silence: Performance structures in sentence production. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 58–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., & Goldberg, A. (1999). Structural priming: Purely syntactic? In M. Hahn & S. Stones (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 208–111). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartsuiker, R., Pickering, M., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Psychological Science, 15, 409–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loebell, H., & Bock, K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41, 791–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M., Pearlmutter, N., & Seidenberg, M. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2004). Multiple solutions to the logical problem of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 31, 883–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonough, K. (2006). Interaction and syntactic priming: English L2 speakers’ production of dative constructions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 179–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, P., & Fox Tree, J. (2003). Building syntactic structures in speaking: a bilingual exploration. Experimental Psychology, 50, 184–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. (1994). Sentence Parsing. In M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 375–409). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W. (2005). Syntactic carpentry: An emergentist approach to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. (1971). Where do sentences come from? In D. Steinberg & L. Jacobovits (Eds.), Semantics (pp. 497–529). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M., & Branigan, H. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M., & Branigan, H. (1999). Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 136–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, M., & Lombardi, L. (1988). Syntactic priming in immediate recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Garrod, S., & Perfetti, C. (1992). Discourse influences during parsing are delayed. Cognition, 45, 109–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1993). Testing a non-decompositional theory of lemma retrieval in speaking: Retrieval of verbs. Cognition, 47, 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz de Mendoza, F., & Mairal, R. (2008). Levels of description and constraining factors in meaning construction: An introduction to the lexical constructional model. Folia Linguistica, 42 (2), 355–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. (2006). Lexical activation of cross-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 299–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. N. (2007). The representation of grammatical gender in the bilingual lexicon: Evidence from Greek and German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 257–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheutz, M., & Eberhard, K. (2004). Effects of morpho-syntactic gender features in bilingual language processing. Cognitive Science, 28, 559–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R., & Pickering, M. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 153–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trueswell, J., Tanenhaus, M., & Garnsey, S. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: the declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 105–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hell, J., & De Groot, A. (1998). Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 193–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Valin, R. Jr. (2005). Exploring the syntax-semantics interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annalisa Baicchi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Baicchi, A. (2015). Concluding Remarks. In: Construction Learning as a Complex Adaptive System. SpringerBriefs in Education. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-18269-8_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics