Skip to main content

Cognition Beyond the Body: Using ABM to Explore Cultural Ecosystems

  • Chapter
Agent-Based Simulation of Organizational Behavior

Abstract

Cognitive science increasingly strives to avoid the gratuitous assumption that minds “represent” a world. For Anthony Chemero, a radical and embodied approach hypothesizes that agent–environment interactions ground all cognitive powers. Pursuing this bold view, the paper shows how agent-based modeling (ABM) can clarify how cultural resources (e.g., sound patterns) enable flexible adaptive behavior. They grant communities modes of action that arise as bodies sensitize to coordinated behavior (e.g. how /a/ is spoken). Using ABM, Stanford and Kenny (Language Variation and Change, 25(02), 119–153, 2013) examine pronunciation shifts. They show how future changes can be prefigured in simple child-agents: this pinpoints the premature theorization that all too often bedevils the human sciences. Given significant differences in how /a/ is pronounced, many mistakenly conclude that there must be an “underlying” (neural) mechanism. They ignore the diachronic nature of human cognition—much depends on history. Those who immediately posit inner mechanisms stumble into the e-bar () fallacy: they assume that an intervening variable (or system) must explain any significant difference. ABM is thus a deflationary weapon to investigate cognition beyond the brain. Pursuing the positive agenda, I echo Robert Rosen in stressing that biological encoding is creative: models can show how social norms empower diachronic systems. While based in embrained bodies, humans depend on ascribing sense to events in cognitive cultural ecosystems. As living beings, persons use artifacts, institutions and the said—they exploit impersonal resources. A major advantage of ABM is that it can be used to model how such resources enrich body-based cognition.

Simulation is a thirdway of doing science.

Robert Axelrod,1997

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Hutchins (1995) famously declares: “Culture is not any collection of things, whether tangible or abstract. Rather, it is a process. It is a human cognitive process that takes place inside and outside the minds of people. It is a process in which our everyday cultural practices are enacted.” (1995: 354) Because culture adds value to human life, its adaptive products, cultural ecosystems, favor modes of action (e.g., flying planes, navigation, and blogging): designers and those who come before exert direct effects on those who follow.

  2. 2.

    Conte, Andrighetto, and Campennì (2013) define immergence in terms of a recursive dimension of emergence where information flows from the social environment back into the individual. While they are interested in the internalization of cultural norms, I pursue the role of immergence in a cultural ecosystem where individuals from an earlier period exert a direct influence on those who follow them. Reading “God has no pleasure in the death of the wicked” brings forth echoes of the Biblical figure of Ezequiel (for discussion, see Markoš, Švorcová, & Lhotský 2013). This is taken to mesh the use of statistical evidence with how agents self-configure by using agent-internal processes (and, in living systems, sensorimotor activity).

  3. 3.

    I use “observe” in roughly Maturana’s (2002) sense (for discussion, see Raimondi 2014): extending second-order cybernetics, he traces the bio-logic of the said to the recursive structural coupling of human languaging of the “doings of the observer.” In saying anything at all, the said thus takes on a subjective aspect. As an anonymous referee points out, this sets up a parallel between saying and using an ABM: in both cases an attentional technology (a model or a verbal pattern) brings about non-random events—a way of saying or a “chance” to see new aspects.

  4. 4.

    While, for Wittgenstein’s philosophical purposes, this is enough, a fuller view of how aspects are perceived would consider the “pre-conceptual” too. In modeling, this would be a necessary basis.

  5. 5.

    As a cultural ecosystem, blogging is cultural process where those who came before (and one’s previous self) exert direct effects on later blogs (and one’s later self). As in a cockpit, the blogger uses a designed environment where writing adds value to writing in ways that do not apply in, say, writing the same words on a scrap of paper.

  6. 6.

    Uryu, Steffensen, and Kramsch (2014) describe a case where a Japanese visitor to America tries to help a German partner who is fishing for a delicate expression by saying that the then Pope had been a Nazi.

  7. 7.

    In the frog’s ecosystem, assumptions are “correct” where applicable to observations of frog populations in the wild or within the frame of ethology. Further, as an anonymous referee points out, many models assume, for example, that agents are rational actors: while the assumption appears zany as applied to living human beings, as economics shows, the results can characterize populations.

  8. 8.

    On a mainstream or code view, brains or minds are said to use inner words coded by a phonemic (or quasi alphabetic) system. Often this is called a “mental lexicon.”

  9. 9.

    Van Orden, Holden, and Turvey (2003) report on reaction times of ten subjects over 1,100 trials responded to a visual signal where they uttered /ta/ into a microphone. Similar patterns of variation arise in ten, a hundred and a thousand trials. The authors conclude that the fractal “signature of variation in the laboratory performance is the dynamical signature of intentional behavior” (p. 338).

  10. 10.

    At the risk of being pedantic, a person who reports a phonetic pattern [kæt] is said to hear /kæt/ (i.e., what is spelled “cat”). Yet, where an outside observer hears an instance of “cat” as, say, [kɛt], an interlocutor may not notice the detail. He or she may, for example, be getting a cat off a table. In any case, there is no reason to suppose that she (or her brain) represents a form /kæt/; simply, like [kɛt], /kæt/ is a second-order description of how exemplars of “cat” can be spoken, heard, and reported.

References

  • Axelrod, R. (1997). Advancing the art of simulation in the social sciences. In Simulating social phenomena (pp. 21–40). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brier, S. (2008). Cybersemiotics: Why information is not enough! Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. (1994). A view of phonology from a cognitive and functional perspective. Cognitive Linguistics, 5, 285–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2011). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conte, R., Andrighetto, G., & Campenni, M. (Eds.). (2013). Minding norms: Mechanisms and dynamics of social order in agent societies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. (2011a). Taking a language stance. Ecological Psychology, 23(3), 185–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. (2011b). Distributed language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, S. J. (2014). Linguistic embodiment and verbal constraints: Human cognition and the scales of time. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1085. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, F. (2014). Voice, (inter-) subjectivity, and real time recurrent interaction. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 760. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1989). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, B. H. (2007). Values define fields: The intentional dynamics of driving, carrying, leading, negotiating, and conversing. Ecological Psychology, 19(2), 153–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 34–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kravchenko, A. (2008). Biology of cognition and linguistic analysis: From non-realist linguistics to a realistic language science. Frankfurt am Main, Berlin: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravchenko, A. V. (2007). Essential properties of language, or, why language is not a code. Language Sciences, 29(5), 650–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (2007). Transmission and diffusion. Language, 83, 344–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linell, P. (2004). The written language bias in linguistics: Its nature, origins and transformations. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Love, N. (2004). Cognition and the language myth. Language Sciences, 26(6), 525–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markoš, A., Švorcová, J., & Lhotský, J. (2013). Living as languaging: Distributed knowledge in living beings. In S. J. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain (pp. 71–92). London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R. (1978). Biology of language: The epistemology of reality (pp. 27–63).

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R. (2002). Autopoiesis, structural coupling and cognition: A history of these and other notions in the biology of cognition. Cybernetics and Human Knowing, 9, 5–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2010). Introduction to the special issue on 4E cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 459–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R. (1978). Biology of language: The epistemology of reality. In G. A. Miller & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Psychology and biology of language and thought: Essays in honor of Eric Lenneberg (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, M., & Cowley, S. J. (2015). Criminal organisations and society: Learning from the Mafia. In D. Secchi & M. Neumann (Eds.), Agent-based simulation of organizational behavior: New frontiers of social science research. New York: Springer (this volume).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, J. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition, and contrast. In J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency effects and the emergence of lexical structure (pp. 137–157). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Port, R. (2007). How are words stored in memory? Beyond phones and phonemes. New Ideas in Psychology, 25(2), 143–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raimondi, V. (2014). Social interaction, languaging and the operational conditions for the emergence of observing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 899. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, R. (1991). Life itself: A comprehensive inquiry into the nature, origin, and fabrication of life. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, UK: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheutz, M., Madey, G., & Boyd, S. (2005). tMANS – The multi-scale agent-based networked simulation for the study of multi-scale, multi-level biological and social phenomena. In Proceedings of Spring Simulation Multiconference (SMC 05), Agent-Directed Simulation Symposium.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanford, J. N., & Kenny, L. A. (2013). Revisiting transmission and diffusion: An agent-based model of vowel chain shifts across large communities. Language Variation and Change, 25(02), 119–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffensen, S. V. (2012). Care and conversing in dialogical systems. Language Sciences, 34(5), 513–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torre, E. (2014). Digital inscriptions as material anchors for future action: Multi-scalar integration and dynamic systems. Cybernetics & Human Knowing, 21(1–2), 128–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uryu, M., Steffensen, S. V., & Kramsch, C. (2014). The ecology of intercultural interaction: Timescales, temporal ranges and identity dynamics. Language Sciences, 41, 41–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(3), 331–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. W. (1957). Philosophical investigations (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfram, W., & Schilling-Estes, N. (1998). American English: Dialects and variation. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

While, the paper’s flaws draw on how humanity arises through the humanity of others, its merits owe much to two people. First, Martin Neumann both taught me about modeling and, in so doing, spurred me to extend a systemic view of cognition. Second, Sabine Thürmel, sought clarification about observers—why the human is no product of swarm intelligence. I still owe her a convincing answer. Sorry, Sabine. However, let the paper be a start; having constructed what stands here, I offer gratitude to both of them and my inspiring reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen J. Cowley .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cowley, S.J. (2016). Cognition Beyond the Body: Using ABM to Explore Cultural Ecosystems. In: Secchi, D., Neumann, M. (eds) Agent-Based Simulation of Organizational Behavior. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-18153-0_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics