Skip to main content

Abstract

The ability to use a scientific concept is intimately linked to the ability to “fluently juggle with its verbal, mathematical, and visual/graphical aspects, applying whichever is most appropriate in the moment and freely translating back and forth among them” (Lemke 1994). This translation across multiple modes of representation places a high demand on cognitive resources, particularly for young children. This chapter details a project that examines the effects of using an SWH approach with younger children (K-3rd grade). Most projects that have examined the effects of SWH have focused on older children (i.e., 3rd grade and older). As part of the project, a coding scheme was developed to better understand what modes of representation children this age used to represent scientific concepts, whether and how these modes were linked to one another, and how these modes and links varied by grade level. In addition to the examination of these child level outcomes, key indicators of teachers’ implementation of the approach were studied in another early childhood sample. In the second project, we explored the classroom environment conditions in relation to the use of language that can promote young children’s engagement in science when using the SWH approach. By using the framework of learning about language, through using language as you live the language of science the research was able to explore what are some of the crucial language features teachers engage with in promoting understanding of the science. In particular the study focused on the difference between teachers with 2 years of experience vs teachers with 3 years of experience using the SWH approach.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aguirre-Mendez, C. P., Yoon, S., Keles, N., Linebarger, D. L., & Hand, B. M. (2013, March). Early learners’ multiple representation in the context of the science writing heuristic approach. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Pittsburg, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akkus, R., Gunel, M., & Hand, B. (2007). Comparing an inquiry based approach known as the Science Writing Heuristic to traditional science teaching practices: Are there differences? International Journal of Science Education, 29, 1745–1765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. Media Psychology, 3(3), 265–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., & Brito, N. (2013). From specificity to flexibility: Developmental changes during infancy. In P. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 453–479). Chichester: Wiley and Sons.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., & Schwartz, D. L. (1999). Rethinking transfer: A simple proposal with multiple implications. In A. Iran-Nejad & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Review of research in education (Vol. 24, pp. 61–100). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, K., Greenbowe, T., & Hand, B. (2006). Implementing the Science Writing Heuristic in the chemistry laboratory. Journal of Chemical Education, 83, 1032–1038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conezio, K., & French, L. (2002). Science in the preschool classroom: Capitalizing on children’s fascination with the everyday world to foster language and literacy development. Young Children, 57(5), 12–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, C., Goodman, N. D., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition, 120, 341–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W., Kuhn, D., & Siegler, R. (1998). Handbook of child psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danish, J. A., & Phelps, D. (2011). The interactional role of kindergarten and first grade students’ representational practices. Paper presented at The Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fantuzzo, J., Sekino, Y., & Cohen, H. L. (2004). An examination of the contributions of interactive peer play to salient classroom competencies for urban Head Start children. Psychology in the Schools, 41(3), 323–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. J., & Forman, E. A. (2006). Chapter 1: Redefining disciplinary learning in classroom contexts. Review of Research in Education, 30(1), 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy implications. Science, 337(6102), 1623–1627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. London: Arnod Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Martin, J. R. (1993). Writing science: Literacy and discursive power. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hand, B. (Ed.). (2008). Science inquiry, argument and language: The case for the Science Writing Heuristic (SWH). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hand, B., & Keys, C. (1999). Inquiry investigation. The Science Teacher, 66(4), 27–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hand, B., Norton-Meier, L., Gunel, M., & Akkus, R. (2015). Aligning teaching and learning: A 3–year study of embedding authentic language and science practices within elementary science classrooms. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, pp. 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10763-015-9622-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayne, H. (2006). Bridging the gap: The relation between learning and memory during infancy. In M. H. Johnson & Y. Munakata (Eds.), Attention and performance XXI: Processes of change in brain and cognitive development (pp. 209–231). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. (2011). Hide and seek and the air in the closet: Environments for learning. In B. Hand & L. Norton-Meier (Eds.), Voices from the classroom: Elementary teachers’ experience with argument-based inquiry. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozma, R. (1994). Will media influence learning: Reframing the debate. Educational Technology Research and Development, 42, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G. R. (1997). Before writing: Rethinking the paths to literacy. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy. In J. L. R. Rubenstein & P. Rakic (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Neural circuit development and function in the brain (Vol. 3, pp. 231–248). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. (1994). The missing context in science education: Science. Paper presented at American Educational Research Association annual meeting, Atlanta, April 1992. Arlington: ERIC Documents Service (ED 363 511), 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malin, J. (2011). What’s the big ideas? Putting concept maps into the hands of your students. In B. Hand & L. Norton-Meier (Eds.), Voices from the classroom: Elementary teachers’ experience with argument-based inquiry. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, S. (2011). Science argumentation and the arts. In B. Hand & L. Norton-Meier (Eds.), Voices from the classroom: Elementary teachers’ experience with argument-based inquiry. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (2003). How literacy in its fundamental sense is central to scientific literacy. Science Education, 87, 224–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton-Meier, L. (2008). Creating border convergence between science and language: A case for the Science Writing Heuristic. In B. Hand (Ed.), Science inquiry, argument and language: The case for the Science Writing Heuristic (SWH) (pp. 13–24). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton-Meier, L., Hand, B., Cavagnetto, A., Akkus, R., & Gunel, M. (2009). Pedagogy, implementation and professional development for teaching science literacy: How students and teacher know and learn. In M. C. Shelley II, L. D. Yore, & B. Hand (Eds.), Quality research in literacy and science education: International perspectives and gold standards (pp. 169–188). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keeffe, M. (2010). Media and the making of scientists. Unpublished dissertation. University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia. pp. 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10763-015-9622-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pahl, K., & Rowsell, J. (Eds.). (2006). Travel notes from the new literacy studies: Instances of practice. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richmond, J., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). Accounting for change in declarative memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review, 27, 349–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, J. (2011). Implementing science conversations with young learners. In B. Hand & L. Norton-Meier (Eds.), Voices from the classroom: Elementary teachers’ experience with argument-based inquiry. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawada, D., Piburn, M., Judson, E., Turley, J., Falconer, K., Benford, R., & Bloom, I. (2002). Measuring reform practices in science and mathematics classrooms: The reformed teaching observation protocol. School Science and Mathematics, 102(6), 245–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E. (1999). Approaches to representation as a psychological construct: A treatise in diversity. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Development of mental representation: Theories and applications (pp. 3–12). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, D. S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2013). Guided play: Where curricular goals meet a playful pedagogy. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7(2), 104–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yelland, N. J., O’Rourke, M. E., Lee, L., & Harrison, C. (2008). Rethinking learning in early childhood education. Buckingham: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, S. (2012). Dual processing and discourse space: Exploring fifth grade students’ language, reasoning, and understanding through writing. Unpublished dissertation, University of Iowa, Iowa City.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Deborah L. Linebarger .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Linebarger, D.L., Norton-Meier, L. (2016). Scientific Concepts, Multiple Modalities, and Young Children. In: Hand, B., McDermott, M., Prain, V. (eds) Using Multimodal Representations to Support Learning in the Science Classroom. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16450-2_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16450-2_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-16449-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-16450-2

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics