Skip to main content

Lexeme Formation in a Conscious Approach to the Lexicon

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Semantics of Complex Words

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 3))

Abstract

N + N compounds in Italian as well as in other Romance languages are differently judged in the current literature. Most scholars agree that only a subset should be treated as true compounds, while the rest is mostly rejected. However, it is not clear what the right criteria are for assessing their status as compounds or as syntactic units. In this paper, an entirely morphological approach will be advocated, which benefits from a careful distinction of two different senses of the term lexicon: the Bloomfieldian sense of the lexicon intended as a repository, the Lexicon1 and the morphological lexicon intended as the set of potential (regularly derived or compounded) lexemes of a language, the Lexicon2. The consistently morphological approach will help us shed light on the intricate issue of Romance compounding invoking general principles for keeping what results from a syntactic pattern and is likely to be a more or less entrenched unit, i.e. a Lex1-lexeme, and what is likely to be produced by an abstract morphological pattern, i.e. a Lex2-lexeme.

Parts of this paper were presented at the 15th International Morphology Meeting (Vienna 9.-12.2.2012) and at the Workshop “Das Wort als Einheit: Grundlagen und Grenzfälle”, held during the XXXIII Deutscher Romanistentag (Würzburg 22.-25.9.2013). I thank all people present on these occasions as well as two anonymous reviewers for valuable suggestions and remarks. Needless to say, I am solely responsible for views expressed and mistakes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I thank one anonymous reviewer for pointing out this to me.

  2. 2.

    The type (1c) will be contrasted with the so-called identificational type in Sect. 4 below.

  3. 3.

    [Compounding has developed out of the syntactic combination of several words]. This statement must be understood in a genetic perspective, as Paul himself clarifies immediately thereafter: “Dies ist nicht so zu verstehen, daß jedes einzelne Wort, das wir als Zusammensetzung betrachten, so entstanden wäre, vielmehr, nachdem eine Anzahl syntaktischer Verbindungen zu einer Worteinheit verschmolzen waren, wirkten dieselben als Zusammensetzungen, nicht mehr als syntaktische Verbindungen, analogisch weiter” [This is not to be understood as if every single word that we regard as a compound came into being in this way, but rather after a certain number of syntactic combinations were fused into a word unit the latter – not felt any longer as syntactic combinations – acted analogically as compounds upon further words]. This view of the genesis of compounding was well established in the diachronically oriented paradigm of the nineteenth century linguistics (cf. Kastovsky 2009). It is interesting to observe that this view is perfectly compatible with the current constructional paradigm which regards abstract patterns as emergent from the entrenchment of concrete chunks (cf. Gaeta 2008 for a discussion).

  4. 4.

    See in this regard Scalise and Guevara (2005: 147): “The term Lexicalism refers to the theoretical standpoint in modern generative linguistics according to which the processes that form complex words (derivation and compounding) are accounted for by a set of Lexical Rules, independent of and different from the syntactic rules of the grammar (i.e. word formation is not performed by syntactic transformations). Such Lexical Rules are assumed to operate in a presyntactic component, the Lexicon”.

  5. 5.

    Incidentally, this is also the null hypothesis more or less explicitly assumed in gammaticalization studies, whereby affixes go back to the grammaticalization of earlier lexemes repeatedly occurring in certain syntactic environments (cf. Hopper and Traugott 2003: 100). Therefore, it is not only compounding which genetically results from the univerbation of syntactic combinations, but it is more in general morphology which comes to being from the routinization of earlier syntactic combinations.

  6. 6.

    Other criteria listed by Lieber and Štekauer (2009) focus on phonological (presence of a specific stress pattern) and morphological (presence of linking elements) properties which will be discussed in Sect. 3. Finally, I fully agree upon the scarce role played by spelling as also pointed out by the authors.

  7. 7.

    Cf. Corbin (1997: 59): “[L]a morphologie a davantage vocation à construire des unités lexicales que les autres composants de la grammaire … Mais … ses produits ne sont pas automatiquement lexicalisés” [Morphology has more vocation to build lexical units than the other components of grammar. But its products are not automatically lexicalized].

  8. 8.

    The model of Construction Morphology as elaborated by Booij (2010) can be surely adopted to represent the views expressed here. However, I find this model not entirely satisfactory because of the lack of a clear distinction between Lex1 and Lex2, as is shown by the interpretation provided for the Lexical Integrity Principle which assumes as is known that “[t]he syntax neither manipulates nor has access to the internal structure of words” (Booij 2010: 99). In this regard, Booij (2009: 97) observes that “[t]he main reason why we consider a sequence of morphemes a word is that that sequence behaves as a cohesive unit with respect to syntactic processes. In other words, cohesiveness is the defining criterion for canonical wordhood, whereas other properties such as being a listeme (a conventional expression) are clearly not to be seen as defining properties for wordhood. Hence, if we take the notion word seriously, we might say that its defining property is cohesiveness or non-interruptability”. However, this alleged divide line between syntactic and morphological constructions consisting in the property of cohesiveness has been shown in Sect. 2 above to be unreliable for distinguishing between Lex1- and Lex2-lexemes.

  9. 9.

    Other cases like camposanto ‘cemetery, lit. field-holy’ occur which displays only a final plural marking camposanti / *campisanti, but this seems rather to be due to a general process which also concerns former syntactic units like pomo d’oro ‘tomato, lit. apple of gold’ / pl. pomi d’oro undergoing a process of univerbation: pomodoro / pomodori. Thus, this process of externalization of inflection seems to generally characterize Lex1-lexemes.

  10. 10.

    On the other hand, the process of externalization of inflection can also affect single compounds testifying of their acquired Lex1-status, as for instance capostazione ‘station master’/capostazioni besides capistazione. As is typical with Lex1, the occurrence of this process is not foreseeable a priori, as shown by other compounds like capo partito ‘party leader’/capi partito/*capopartiti, etc. (see Gaeta and Ricca 2009 for more details).

  11. 11.

    For brevity, I will not discuss Scalise and Bisetto’s (2009) further categorization of these types.

  12. 12.

    The logic of this argument is not entirely clear to me because metaphoric meanings also play an important role in many syntactic environments. For instance, in Italian as well as in other Romance languages an adjective can precede a noun only if it allows a metaphoric interpretation: un amaro caffé ‘a bitter coffee’ vs. un caffé amaro ‘a coffee without sugar’, but *un ghiacciato caffé vs. un caffé ghiacciato ‘an ice-cold coffee’.

  13. 13.

    Notice that the filter can be overcome insofar as the Lex1-lexeme is more stabilized: for instance, in the case of the univerbation pomodoro seen above the filter is completely switched off giving rise to derivatives like pomodor-ino ‘tomato-dim’. Moreover, the Filter-Principle can also be overcome any time a particularly expressive Lex1-lexeme enters a lexeme formation pattern, as shown for instance by the derivative celodurista ‘male chauvinist’, which is based on the fully specified idiom Ce l’ho duro! ‘I have a hard-on!, lit. I’ve got it hard’.

References

  • Arnaud, Pierre J.L. 2003. Les composés timbre-poste. Lyons: pul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, Mark. 1994. Morphology by itself. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baroni, Marco, Emiliano Guevara, and Roberto Zamparelli. 2009. The dual nature of deverbal nominal constructions. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 5(1): 27–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, Laurie. 1998. When is a sequence of two nouns a compound in English? English Language and Linguistics 2(1): 65–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, Laurie. 2001. Morphological productivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, Laurie. 2010. The typology of exocentric compounding. In Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding, ed. Sergio Scalise and Irene Vogel, 167–175. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, Melanie J. 2011. At the boundary of morphology and syntax. Noun noun constructions in English. In Morphology and its interfaces, ed. Alexandra Galani, Glyn Hicks, and George Tsoulas, 137–168. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bisetto, Antonietta. 2004. Composizione con elementi italiani. In La formazione delle parole in italiano, ed. Maria Grossmann and Franz Rainer, 31–50. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisetto, Antonietta, and Sergio Scalise. 1999. Compounding: Morphology and/or syntax? In Boundaries of morphology and syntax, ed. Lunella Mereu, 31–48. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Bejamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 2009. Lexical integrity as a formal universal: A constructionist view. In Universals of language universal today, ed. Sergio Scalise, Antonietta Bisetto, and Elisabetta Magni, 83–100. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 2012. Construction morphology and the interaction of syntax and word formation. In Los límites de la morfología. Estudios ofrecidos a Soledad Varela Ortega, ed. Antonio Fábregas, Elena Felíu, Josefa Martín, and José Pazó, 105–113. Madrid: UAM Ediciones.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbin, Danielle. 1992. Hypothèses sur les frontières de la composition nominale. Cahiers de grammaire 17: 25–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbin, Danielle. 1997. Locutions, composés, unités polylexématiques: lexicalisation et mode de construction. In La locution entre langue et usages, ed. Michel Martins-Baltar, 53–101. Fontenay: ENS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coseriu, Eugenio. 1978. Sincronía, diacronía e historia. El problema del cambio lingüístico, 3rd ed. Madrid: Gredos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Sciullo, Anna M. 2005. Decomposing compounds. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics 2(3): 14–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, Wolfgang U. 1988. Preferences vs. strict universals in morphology: Word-based rules. In Theoretical morphology, ed. Michael Hammond and Michael Noonan, 143–153. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, Wolfgang U., and Lavinia Merlini Barbaresi. 1994. Morphopragmatics. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fradin, Bernard. 2009. IE, Romance: French. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Štekauer, 417–435. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. 2003. Ai limiti della morfologia basata sulle parole. In Scritti di morfologia. In onore di Sergio Scalise in occasione del suo 60° compleanno, ed. Antonietta Bisetto, Claudio Iacobini, and Anna M. Thornton, 47–59. Roma: Caissa Italia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. 2006. Lexical integrity as a constructional strategy. Lingue e Linguaggio 5(1): 67–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. 2007. On the double nature of productivity in inflectional morphology. Morphology 17(2): 181–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. 2008. Constituent order in compounds and syntax: Typology and diachrony. Morphology 18(2): 117–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. 2011. La composizione in italiano e oltre. In Scuola di formazione di italiano lingua seconda/straniera: competenze d’uso e integrazione, ed. Michela Cennamo and Annamaria Lamarra, 89–108. Napoli: ESI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio. to appear. Restrictions in word formation. In Handbook of word-formation. An international handbook of the languages of Europe, ed. Peter O. Müller, Ohnheiser Ingeborg, Olsen Susan, and Rainer Franz. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio, and Davide Ricca. 2006. Productivity in Italian word formation: A variable-corpus approach. Linguistics 44(1): 57–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaeta, Livio, and Davide Ricca. 2009. Composita solvantur: Compounds as lexical units or morphological objects? Italian Journal of Linguistics 21(1): 35–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, Adele E. 2013. Constructionist approaches to language. In Handbook of construction grammar, ed. Thomas Hoffmann and Graeme Trousdale, 15–31. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, Maria, and Franz Rainer. 2009. Italian adjective-adjective compounds: Between morphology and syntax. Italian Journal of Linguistics 21(1): 71–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, Paul J., and Elizabeth C. Traugott. 2003. Grammaticalization, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kastovsky, Dieter. 2009. Diachronic perspectives. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Štekauer, 323–340. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieber, Rochelle, and Pavol Štekauer. 2009. Introduction: Status and definition of compounding. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Štekauer, 3–18. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masini, Francesca. 2009. Phrasal lexemes, compounds and phrases: A constructionist perspective. Word Structure 2(2): 254–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masini, Francesca, and Sergio Scalise. 2012. Italian compounds. Probus 24: 61–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montermini, Fabio. 2008. Il lato sinistro della morfologia. Milano: Angeli.

    Google Scholar 

  • NGLE. 2009. Nueva gramática de la lengua española. Morfología, Syntaxis I, vol. I. Madrid: Real Academia Española/Asociación de Academias de la Lengua Española.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, Hermann. 1920/1995. Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte, 10th ed. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John, and Rodney Huddleston. 2002. Nouns and noun phrases. In The Cambridge grammar of the English language, ed. Rodney Huddleston and Geoffrey K. Pullum, 323–523. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rainer, Franz. 1988. Towards a theory of blocking: The case of Italian and German quality nouns. In Yearbook of morphology 1988, ed. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 155–185. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rainer, Franz. 1993. Spanische Wortbildungslehre. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rainer, Ranz, and Soledad Varela. 1992. Compounding in Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica 4(1): 117–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricca, Davide. 2010. Corpus data and theoretical implications: With special reference to Italian V-N compounds. In Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding, ed. Sergio Scalise and Irene Vogel, 237–254. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, Sergio, and Antonietta Bisetto. 2008. La struttura delle parole. Bologna: Il Mulino.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, Sergio, and Antonietta Bisetto. 2009. The classification of compounds. In The Oxford handbook of compounding, ed. Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Štekauer, 34–53. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise, Sergio, and Emiliano Guevara. 2005. The lexicalist approach to word-formation and the notion of the lexicon. In Handbook of word-formation, ed. Pavol Štekauer and Rochelle Lieber, 147–187. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, Martin. 2009. A + N constructions in Mandarin and the ‘compound versus phrase’ debate. Word Structure 2(2): 272–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultink, Henk. 1961. Produktiviteit als morfologisch fenomeen. Forum der Letteren 2: 110–125.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Livio Gaeta .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gaeta, L. (2015). Lexeme Formation in a Conscious Approach to the Lexicon. In: Bauer, L., Körtvélyessy, L., Štekauer, P. (eds) Semantics of Complex Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 3. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-14102-2_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics