Skip to main content

The Roles of Phonology in Silent Reading: A Selective Review

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 46))

Abstract

This chapter presents a selective review of evidence about how phonological representations are involved in silent reading. Knowledge of the mapping of orthography onto phonology appears to be important in skilled reading, and this knowledge is applied very early in the process of recognizing words in isolation. The same is true when one is reading sentences and texts, and the creation of a phonological representation of a text appears to play a critical role in guiding the movement of the eyes during reading. Phonological representations beyond the level of the individual word, including prosodic representations, also seem to play an important role in guiding parsing and in integrating discourse.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Ashby (2006) found that this facilitation was limited to low-frequency words, suggesting that phonological processing may play a smaller role in recognizing very familiar words.

  2. 2.

    Breen and Clifton 2014 in press, reported that when parafoveal preview of the disambiguating word was prevented, the disruption appeared later, after the disambiguating word was actually fixated.

  3. 3.

    See Jun (2010) for evidence that overt prosody may not have the properties assumed by Fodor’s implicit prosody hypothesis (IPH). In particular, English readers tended to place a prosodic boundary immediately before a relative clause (RC) in an NP–NP–RC configuration. However, as Jun notes, implicit prosody may well be different. See also Jun (this volume).

  4. 4.

    To be sure, factors other than length affect the resolution of the relative clause ambiguity, most saliently, whether the modified noun phrase (NP) is subject or object of a sentence. In the Hemforth et al. research, the often-discussed differences among languages were rather minor, and could be attributed to factors such as how the different languages encode information status.

  5. 5.

    This latter assumption is apparently inconsistent with Clifton and Staub’s (2008) review of the syntactic processing literature that concluded that syntactic ambiguity has no effect on reading time, or even speeds it. It is possible that prosodic disambiguation has a different effect than contextual or morpho-syntactic disambiguation, or that the demand characteristics of the present experiment induced readers to attempt to use the prosody of the preceding speech melody to resolve the ambiguity of the target sentence.

References

  • Alexander, J. D., & Nygaard, L. C. (2008). Reading voices and hearing text: Talker-specific auditory imagery in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 446–459. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J. (2006). Prosody in skilled silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 29 (3), 318–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J. (2010). Phonology is fundamental in skilled reading: Evidence from ERPs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 95–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2005). The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition, 96(3), B89–100. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Martin, A. (2008). Prosodic phonological representations early in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34, 224–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Rayner, K. (2004). Representing syllable information during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 391–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Treiman, R., Kessler, B. T., & Rayner, K. (2006). Vowel processing during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32, 416–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Sanders, L. D., & Kingston, J. (2009). Skilled readers begin processing phonological features by 80 ms: Evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 80, 84–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M. (1998). Prosodic influences on reading syntactically ambiguous sentences. In J. Fodor & F. Ferreira (Eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing (pp. 1–46). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Benatar, A., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2014). Newness, givenness and discourse updating: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 1–16. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breen, M., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2011). Stress matters: Effects of anticipated lexical stress on silent reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(2), 153–170. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breen, M., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2014). Stress matters revisited: A display change experiment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.766899.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H., & Pollatsek, A. (1985). Recognizing printed words: A look at current models. In T. G. Waller, D. Besner, & G. E. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 5, pp 2–82). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. (1988). Punctuation and the prosody of written language. Written Communication, 5, 396–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, C., Jr., & Staub, A. (2008). Parallelism and competition in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 234–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, C., Jr., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In R. V. Gompel, M. Fisher, W. Murray, & R. L. Hill (Eds.), Eye movement research: Insights into mind and brain (pp. 341–371). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., & Mitchell, D. C. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the Late Closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition, 30, 73–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1976). Phoneme monitoring reaction time as a function of preceding intonation contour. Perception & Psychophysics, 20, 55–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Fodor, J. A. (1979). Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dilley, L. C., & McAuley, J. D. (2008). Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 294–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drieghe, D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2005). Eye movements and word skipping during reading revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 954–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbow, P. (2012). Vernacular eloquence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimmons, G., & Drieghe, D. (2012). How fast can predictability influence word skipping during reading? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi:10.1037/a0030909.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 285–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (2002a). Prosodic disambiguation in silent reading. In M. Hirotani (Ed.), Proceedings of the North East Linguistics Society (Vol. 32, pp. 112–132). Amherst: GSLA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (2002b). Psycholinguistics cannot escape prosody. Paper presented at Speech Prosody 2002, Aix-en-Provence, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: Rue issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 71–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic information in reading. Journal of experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haber, R. N., & Haber, L. R. (1982). Does silent reading involve articulation? Evidence from tongue-twisters. American Journal of Psychology, 95, 409–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halderman, L. K., Ashby, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2012). Phonology: An early and integral role in identifying words. In J. S. Adelman (Ed.), Visual word recognition: Vol. 1. Models and methods, orthography, and phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 207–228). London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huey, E. B. (1908/1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jun, S.-A. (2010). The implicit prosody hypothesis and overt prosody in English. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(7–9), 1201–1233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kentner, G. (2012). Linguistic rhythm guides parsing decisions in written sentence comprehension. Cognition, 123, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., & Matt, A. M. (1977). If you speak slowly, do people read your prose slowly? Person-particular speech recoding during reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9(4), 250–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1972). Beyond auditory coding. In J. F. Kavanagh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye (pp. 241–248). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo, Y., Yan, M., & Zhou, X. (2012). Prosodic boundaries delay the processing of upcoming lexical information during silent sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 915–930. doi:10.1037/a0029182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1994). Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. (1982). The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 672–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E., Schvaneveldt, R. W., & Ruddy, M. G. (1974). Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word-recognition. Memory & Cognition, 2(2), 309–321. doi:10.3758/BF03209002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Tan, L. H., & Rayner, K. (2000). The role of phonological codes in integrating information across saccadic eye movements in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 607–633. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2011). The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 246–253. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1975). Parafoveal identification during a fixation in reading. Acta Psychologica, 39, 172–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1995). Phonological codes are automatically activated during reading: Evidence from an eye movement paradigm. Psychological Science, 6, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., Ashby, J., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2012). Psychology of reading (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. F., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z Reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 445–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E., Warren, T., & McConnell, K. (2009). Using E-Z Reader to model the effects of higher-level language processing on eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sereno, S. C., & Rayner, K. (1992). Fast priming during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schotter, E. R. (2013). Constraints on semantic preview benefit. Poster presented at the Psychonomic Society, Toronto, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzschild, R. (1999). Givenness, avoid and other constraints on the placement of accent. Natural Language Semantics, 7, 141–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slowiaczek, M. L., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1980). Subvocalization and reading for meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19(5), 573–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparison of speech and typerwriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 117–152). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 371–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walter, M., Clifton, C., Jr., Frazier, L., Hemforth, B., Konieczny, L., & Seelig, H. (1999). Prosodic and syntactic effects on relative clause attachment in German and English. Poster presented at AMLaP 1999, Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, S., & Morris, R. K. (2009). Phonological similarity effects in reading. Paper presented at the European Conference on Eye Movements, Southampton, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yao, B., & Scheepers, C. (2011). Contextual modulation of reading rate for direct versus indirect speech quotations. Cognition, 121(3), 447–453. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Clifton Jr. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Clifton, C. (2015). The Roles of Phonology in Silent Reading: A Selective Review. In: Frazier, L., Gibson, E. (eds) Explicit and Implicit Prosody in Sentence Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 46. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12961-7_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics