Skip to main content

Time and Adherence: A Principle of Foresight

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 777 Accesses

Part of the book series: Philosophy and Medicine ((PHME,volume 118))

Abstract

When we consider two possible and contradictory actions, their adjacent desires have corresponding rewards, which might not be available at the same time. Thus time influences our decision , leading to what is described as intertemporal choice : Often, in chronic diseases, the choice of adherence or nonadherence can be seen as a choice between an abstract and distant reward, maintaining health, and a near and concrete reward, for example the pleasure of smoking. Many people are naturally impatient, preferring a small, near reward to a large, distant reward. This trait, patient or impatient, may be linked to adherence. I propose that, in the particular case of akrasia represented by patient nonadherence to long-term therapies, there is such disequilibrium between the two types of actions, the continent and the incontinent , that it does not allow the principle of continence to express itself, or rather this principle becomes insufficient, or even inappropriate, if used alone. This leads me to propose a hypothesis introducing a second principle, which I call the principle of foresight , which pushes us to give priority to the future , i.e., to accept taking care of ourselves. Maybe we have here something that is essentially human. One can also speculate that this differentiation is accomplished slowly in adults, leading from the simple age of reason to an age of foresight . According to this hypothesis, not conforming to this principle leads to nonadherence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I am grateful to John Meyers for this remark.

  2. 2.

    I am grateful to John Meyers for this remark.

References

  • Ahmed AT, Karter AJ, Liu J. Alcohol consumption is inversely associated with adherence to diabetes self-care behaviours. Diabet Med. 2006;23:795–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie G. Beyond microeconomics, conflict among interests in multiple self as a determinant of value. In: Elster J, editor. The multiple self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie G. Picoeconomics: the strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the Person. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie G. A selectionist model of the ego: implications for self-control. In: Sebanz N, Prinz W, editors. Disorders of volition. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2006. www.picoeconomics.com/Articles/MunichRepr.pdf, 2003.

  • Basil MD, Basil DZ, Schooler C. Cigarette advertising to counter New Year’s resolutions. J Health Commun. 2000;5:161–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister R, Voes K. Willpower, choice and self-control. In: Loeweinstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision, economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergson H. Creative evolution. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickel J, Johnson MW. Delay discounting, a fundamental behavioral process of drug dependence. In: Loewenstein G, Reid D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickel WK, Miller ML, Yi R, Kowal BP, Lindquist DM, Pitcock JA. Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90S:S85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel WK, Yi R, Kowal BP, Gatchalian KM. Cigarette smokers discount past and future rewards symmetrically and more than controls: is discounting a measure of impulsivity? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;96:256–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boettiger CA, Mitchell JM, Tavares VC, Robertson M, Joslyn G, D’Esposito M, Fields HL. Immediate reward bias in humans: fronto-parietal networks and a role for the catechol-O-methyltransferase 158val/val genotype. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14383–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brookhart MA, Patrick AR, Dormuth C, Avorn J, Shrank W, Cadarette SM, Solomon DH. Adherence to lipid-lowering therapy and the use of preventive health services: an investigation of the healthy user effect. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166:348–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canguilhem G. The normal and the pathological (trans: Fawcett C). New York: Zone Books; 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman GB. Time discounting of health outcomes. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russel Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman GB, Elstein AS. Valuing the future: temporal discounting of health and money. Med Decis Making. 1995;15:373–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christakou A, Brammer M, Rubia K. Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. Neuroimage. 2011;54:1344–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis JR, Xi J, Westfall AO, Cheng H, Lyles K, Saag KG, Delzell E. Improving the prediction of medication compliance: the example of bisphosphonates for osteoporosis. Med Care. 2009;47:334–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR. Descartes’ error. Emotions, reason and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R. The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D. Consciousness explained. New York: Back Bay Books; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim E. Philosophy lectures, notes from the Lycée de Sens, 1883–1884 (trans: Gross N, Jones RA). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J. Ulysses and the sirens: a theory of imperfect rationality. Soc Sci Inf. 1977;16:469–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J. In: Elster J, Skog O-J, editors. Getting hooked, rationality and addictions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999a.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J. Davidson on weakness of the will and self deception. In: Hahn LE, editor. The philosophy of Donald Davidson. Open Court; 1999b.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J. Strong feelings, emotion, addiction and human behavior. The 1997 Jean Nicod lectures. A Bradford book. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2000a.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster J. Ulysses unbound. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000b.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel P. Les croyances. In: Notions de philosophie (under the direction of D. Kamboucher). Paris: Editions folio, Gallimard; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankfurt H. The importance of what we care about. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick S. Time preference and personal identity. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russel Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S. Beyond the pleasure principle. New York: Norton Library; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gjelsvik O. Addiction, weakness of the will and relapse. In: Elster J, Skog O-J, editors. Getting hooked, rationality and addiction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurmankin Levy A, Micco E, Putt M, Armstong K. Value for the future and breast cancer-preventive health behaviour. Cancer Epidemiol Biomark Prev. 2006;15:955–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman CP, Polivy J. Dieting as an exercise in behavioral economy. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homer. The Odyssey. Penguin; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holton R. How is strength of will possible? In: Stroud S, Tappolet C, editors. Weakness of will and practical irrationality. Oxford: Clarendon Press; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horwitz RI, Viscoli CM, Berkman L, Donaldson RM, Horwitz SM, Murray CJ, Ransohoff DF, Sindelar J. Treatment adherence and risk of death after myocardial infarction. Lancet. 1990;336:542–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones BA, Landes RD, Yi R, Bickel WK. Temporal horizon: modulation by smoking status and gender. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;104(Suppl 1):S87–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik A. The evolution of patience. In: Loewinstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russel Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalenscher T, Pennartz CMA. Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;84:284–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalis A1, Mojzisch A, Schweizer TS, Kaiser S. Weakness of will, akrasia, and the neuropsychiatry of decision making: an interdisciplinary perspective. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8:402–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laplantine F. Anthropologie de la maladie. Bibliothèque Scientifique Payot; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux J. The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livet P. Émotions et rationalité morale, P. U. F., Collection Sociologies; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manuck SB, Flory JD, Muldoon MF, Ferrel RE. A neurobiology of intertemporal choice. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. Delay of gratification in children. Science. 1989;244:933–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel W, Ayduck O, Mendoza-Denton R. Sustaining delay of gratification over time: a hot-cool systems perspective. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF, editors. Time and decision. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris AD, Boyle DI, McMahon AD, Greene SA, MacDonald TM, Newton RW. Adherence to insulin treatment, glycaemic control, and ketoacidosis in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. The DARTS/MEMO collaboration. Diabetes audit and research in Tayside Scotland. Medicines Monitoring Unit. Lancet. 1997;350:1505–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norcross JC, Mrykalo MS, Blagys MD. Auld lang syne: success predictors, change processes, and self-reported outcomes of New Year’s resolves and nonresolvers. J Clin Psychol. 2002;58:397–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nordenfelt L. Quality of life, health and happiness. Avebury; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nozick R. The nature of rationality. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogien R. La faiblesse de la volonté. P. U. F.; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology. 2008;200:1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pharo P. Raison et civilisation, essai sur les chances de rationalisation morale de la société, Cerf; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proust J. La Nature de la volonté. Folio Essais, Gallimard; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin H, Raineri A, Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991;55:233–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reach G. A novel conceptual framework for understanding adherence to long-term therapies. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2008;2:7–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reach G. Personal view: does an impatience genotype leads to nonadherence to long-term therapies. Diabetologia. 2010;53:1562–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reach G. Obedience and motivation as mechanisms for adherence to medication. A study in obese type 2 diabetic patients. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2011;5:523–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reach G, Michault A, Bihan H, Paulino C, Cohen R, Le Clésiau H. Patients’ impatience is an independent determinant of poor diabetes control. Diabetes Metab. 2011;37:497–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safford MM, Russell L, Suh DC, Roman S, Pogach L. How much time do patients with diabetes spend on self-care? J Am Board Fam Pract. 2005;18:262–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele CM, Josephs RA. Alcohol myopia. Its prized and dangerous effects. Am Psychol. 1990;45:921–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Story GW, Vlaev I, Seymour B, Darzi A, Dolan RJ. Does temporal discounting explain unhealthy behavior? A systematic review and reinforcement learning perspective. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:76. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00076.

  • Wallace M. Future time perspective in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1956;52:240–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wamala S, Merlo J, Bostrom G, Hogstedt C, Agren G. Socioeconomic disadvantage and primary non-adherence with medication in Sweden. Int J Qual Health Care. 2007;19:134–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff MC, Leander JD. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors decrease impulsive behavior as measured by an adjusting delay procedure in the pigeon. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:421–9.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gérard Reach .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Reach, G. (2015). Time and Adherence: A Principle of Foresight. In: The Mental Mechanisms of Patient Adherence to Long-Term Therapies. Philosophy and Medicine, vol 118. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12265-6_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics