Skip to main content

Which Event Properties Matter for Which Cognitive Task?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and Processing

Abstract

Two important questions in the language sciences have been: What portion of event information is accessed and used during sentence processing? What portion of event information is relevant to the grammar of natural languages? An extensive amount of linguistic and psycholinguistic research over the past couple of decades has shown that quite a bit of event information is relevant to online sentence processing, but only a limited amount of information is relevant to the grammars of natural languages. One possible cause for this divergence is that grammar development and language comprehension are carried out by separate systems that are sensitive to different types of information. Another possible explanation is that grammar development and language comprehension are carried out by an integrated system, but the task demands of language learning and the task demands of language comprehension are different. In this chapter, we show that, grammatical systems that seem more “exotic” from the point of view of more well-known languages still make use of a limited set of properties and that these “exotic” languages still obey the same design constraints as more well-known systems; we then briefly report on some computational models of online reading experiments, which demonstrate quite clearly that a distinct and much larger kind of event knowledge is used by the human parser. We propose an explanation for this difference in the use of event knowledge: Grammars and parsers use different kinds of event knowledge because the tasks listeners and grammar learners must perform are quite distinct.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    McKoon and MacFarland (2000) find corpus examples of verbs normally associated with internal causation appearing in transitive uses, but note of these uses that “if something is said to erode a beach, this cannot be just any something—not a person, not a shovel—it must be something that participates intrinsically in erosion, like wind or water.” This finding furthers the notion that there are links between semantic properties and syntactic properties.

  2. 2.

    The data presented in this chapter comes from a preliminary version of the work reported in Roland et al. (2012).

  3. 3.

    See Goldberg (2006) for the “abstract” meaning of argument-structure constructions and the fact that this meaning corresponds closely to the meaning of “general purpose” verbs like put, give, and so forth.

  4. 4.

    However, if, for example, there were a culture where the color green was associated with that culture’s supreme being, and all actions performed by green-colored agents were thus considered to be special, then we might expect that a separate case marking for green agents could arise.

References

  • Altmann, G., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bicknell, K., Elman, J. L., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Kutas, M. (2010). Effects of event knowledge in processing verbal arguments. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 489–505.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1992). Arenas of language use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conklin, K., Koenig, J.-P., & Mauner, G. (2004). The role of specificity in the lexical encoding of participants. Brain & Language, 90, 221–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hoop, H., & Narashiman, B. (2008). Ergative case-marking in Hindi. In H. deHoop & P. de Swart (Eds.), Differential subject marking (pp. 63–78). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deerwester, S., Dumais, S. T., Furnas, G. W., Landauer, T. K., & Harshman, R. (1990). Indexing by latent semantic analysis. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 41(6), 391–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowty, D. (1991). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language, 67, 547–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N. (2000). Kinship verbs. In P. Vogel & B. Comrie (Eds.), Approaches to the typology of word classes (pp. 103–172). Dordrecht: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazdar, G., Klein, E., Pullum, G. K., & Sag, I. A. (1985). Generalized phrase structure grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, J. (2005). Words and structures. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamide, Y., Altmann, G., & Haywood, S. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 1133–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, J.-P., & Davis, A. (2001). Sublexical modality and the structure of lexical semantic representations. Linguistics and Philosophy, 24, 71–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, J.-P., Mauner, G., & Bienvenue, B. (2003). Arguments for adjuncts. Cognition, 89, 67–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, J.-P., & Michelson, K. (2010). Argument structure of Oneida kin terms. International Journal of American Linguistics, 76, 169–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B. (1993). English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (2005). Argument Realization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., Ferretti, T., & Amyote, L. (1997). Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 137–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malchukov, A. (2008). Animacy and asymmetries in differential case marking. Lingua, 118, 203–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patwardhan, S., & Pederson, T. (2006). Using WordNet-based context vectors to estimate the semantic relatedness of concepts. In Proceedings of the EACL 2006 Workshop Making Sense of Sense—Bringing Computational Linguistics and Psycholinguistics (pp. 1–8). Trento, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B. (2008). The English dative alternation: The case for verb sensitivity. Journal of Linguistics, 44, 129–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roland, D., Yun, H., Koenig, J.-P., & Mauner, G. (2012). Semantic similarity, predictability, and models of sentence processing. Cognition, 122(3), 267–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shakthi, P., & Koenig, J.-P. (2009). Hindi aspectual complex predicates. Proceedings of the HPSG ’09 Conference (pp. 276–296). Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shakthi, P. (2012). Hindi complex predicates at the syntax-semantics interface. Doctoral Dissertation, University at Buffalo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E. (1951). Prediction and entropy of printed English. Bell System Technical Journal, 30, 50–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spivey-Knowlton, M., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition, 55, 227–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. E. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yun, H., Mauner, G., & Koenig, J.-P. (2006). Anticipation vs. integration of syntactically infrequent but semantically obligatory arguments. Poster presented at the 19th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, March 23–25, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jean-Pierre Koenig .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Koenig, JP., Roland, D., Yun, H., Mauner, G. (2015). Which Event Properties Matter for Which Cognitive Task?. In: de Almeida, R., Manouilidou, C. (eds) Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and Processing. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10112-5_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics